Mental operations

Mental operations
Mental operations
Mental operations

Approaches and Types
Piagetian
Seriation
Transitivity
Classification
Decentering
Reversibility
Conservation
Psychometric
Cognition
Memory
Divergent production
Convergent production
Evaluation
Cognitive
Attentional focalization
Attentional discarding
Spatial basic operating scheme
Representation
Comparison
Operations of memory
Temporal basic operating scheme
Systemic
Cognitive operations
Practical operations
Affective operations
Expressive operations
Perceptual-motor operations
Regulative operations

Writers
Pierre Janet
Jean Piaget
Joy Paul Guilford
Silvio Ceccato
Giulio Benedetti
Codrin Stefan Tapu

Psychology portal

Mental operations are operations that affect mental contents. Pierre Janet was one of the first using the concept in psychology. Mental operations have been investigated at a developmental level by Jean Piaget, and from a psychometric perspective by J. P. Guilford. There is also a cognitive approach to the subject, as well as a systems view of it.

Contents

History

In 1903, Pierre Janet describes two types of mental operations:[1]

  • reality operations - mental operations under the control of logic;
  • disinterested operations - escaping the control of reason.

Jean Piaget differentiates a preoperational stage, and operational stages of cognitive development, on the basis of presence of mental operations as an adaptation tool.[2]

J. P. Guilford's Structure of Intellect theory comprises up to 180 different intellectual abilities organized along three dimensions—Operations, Content, and Products.[3]

According to most logicians, the three primary mental operations are apprehension (understanding), judgement, and inference.[4]

Apprehension is the mental operation by which an idea is formed in the mind. If you were to think of a sunset or a baseball, the action of forming that picture in your mind is apprehension. The verbal expression of apprehension is called a term.

Judgment is the mental operation by which we predicate something of a subject. Were you to think, "That sunset is beautiful" or "Baseball is the all-American sport" is to make a judgment. The verbal expression of judgment is the statement (or proposition).

Inference (or reasoning) is the mental operation by which we draw conclusions from other information. If you were to think, "I like to look at that sunset, because I enjoy beautiful things, and that sunset is beautiful" you would be reasoning. The verbal expression of reasoning is the logical argument.[4]

Developmental view

Jean Piaget identifies several mental operations of the concrete operational stage of cognitive development:[2]

Mental operations according to Jean Piaget
  • Seriation—the ability to sort objects in an order according to size, shape, or any other characteristic. For example, if given different-shaded objects they may make a color gradient.
  • Transitivity—The ability to recognize logical relationships among elements in a serial order, and perform 'transitive inferences' (for example, If A is taller than B, and B is taller than C, then A must be taller than C).
  • Classification—the ability to name and identify sets of objects according to appearance, size or other characteristic, including the idea that one set of objects can include another.
  • Decentering—where the child takes into account multiple aspects of a problem to solve it. For example, the child will no longer perceive an exceptionally wide but short cup to contain less than a normally-wide, taller cup.
  • Reversibility—the child understands that numbers or objects can be changed, then returned to their original state. For this reason, a child will be able to rapidly determine that if 4+4 equals t, t−4 will equal 4, the original quantity.
  • Conservation—understanding that quantity, length or number of items is unrelated to the arrangement or appearance of the object or items.

Piaget also describes a formal operational stage, with formal operations of abstract thinking: hypothesizing, hypothesis testing, and deduction.[2]

Psychometric view

Guilford's Structure of Intellect

According to J. P. Guilford's Structure of Intellect (SI) theory, an individual's performance on intelligence tests can be traced back to the underlying mental abilities or factors of intelligence. SI theory comprises multiple intellectual abilities organized along three dimensions—Operations, Content, and Products.[5]

  • Operations dimension

SI includes six operations or general intellectual processes: Cognition—The ability to understand, comprehend, discover, and become aware of information. Memory recording—The ability to encode information. Memory retention—The ability to recall information. Divergent production—The ability to generate multiple solutions to a problem; creativity. Convergent production—The ability to deduce a single solution to a problem; rule-following or problem-solving. Evaluation—The ability to judge whether or not information is accurate, consistent, or valid.

  • Content dimension

SI includes five broad areas of information to which the human intellect applies the six operations: Visual—Information perceived through seeing. Auditory—Information perceived through hearing. Kinesthetic -through actions Symbolic—Information perceived as symbols or signs that have no meaning by themselves; e.g., Arabic numerals or the letters of an alphabet. Semantic—Information perceived in words or sentences, whether oral, written, or silently in one's mind. Behavioral—Information perceived as acts of people.

  • Product dimension

As the name suggests, this dimension contains results of applying particular operations to specific contents. The SI model includes six products, in increasing complexity: Units—Single items of knowledge. Classes—Sets of units sharing common attributes. Relations—Units linked as opposites or in associations, sequences, or analogies. Systems—Multiple relations interrelated to comprise structures or networks. Transformations—Changes, perspectives, conversions, or mutations to knowledge. Implications—Predictions, inferences, consequences, or anticipations of knowledge.

Therefore, according to Guilford there are 6 x 5 x 6 = 180 intellectual abilities or factors. Each ability stands for a particular operation in a particular content area and results in a specific product, such as Comprehension of Figural Units or Evaluation of Semantic Implications.[3]

Cognitive view

Mental operations: cognitive view

Following on the footsteps of Silvio Ceccato,[6] Giulio Benedetti describes several types of mental operations:[7]

  • attentional focalization - focusing attention on something;
  • attentional discarding - stopping our attention on an object;
  • spatial basic operating scheme (attentional movement) - passing attention from one part to another of the attentional field;
  • operation of representation - evoking a mental image;
  • operation of comparison;
  • operations of memory;
  • temporal basic operating scheme - variation of attentional focalization.

Systems view

Mental operations: systems view

Taking into account all mental processes, the following types of mental operations have been described:[8]

  • cognitive operations - production and verbalization of images and thoughts;
  • practical operations, pertaining to executive functions;
  • affective operations - affective evaluation of the world and self;
  • expressive operations (emotional expression);
  • perceptual-motor operations (e.g., eye-hand coordination);
  • regulative operations - verbalization of needs, motives and feelings, and self-control.

See also

References

  1. ^ Valsiner, Jaan; van der Veer, René (2000). The social mind: construction of the idea. Cambridge University Press, pp. 103-106. ISBN 0521589738.
  2. ^ a b c Ginsburg, Herbert; Opper, Sylvia (1979). Piaget's Theory of Intellectual Development. Prentice Hall, p. 152. ISBN 0136751407.
  3. ^ a b Guilford, Joy Paul (1980). Some changes in the structure of intellect model. Educational and Psychological Measurement 48: 1-4.
  4. ^ a b Hobhouse, LT (2005). The Theory of Knowledge: A Contribution to Some Problems of Logic and Metaphysics, Kessinger Publishing, p. 223. ISBN 9781417962068.
  5. ^ Guilford, Joy Paul (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist 5 (9): 444-454.
  6. ^ Ceccato, Silvio (1961). Linguistic Analysis and Programming for Mechanical Translation. G. Feltrinelli.
  7. ^ Benedetti, Giulio (2005). "Basic mental operations which make up mental categories" (PDF). www.mind-consciousness-language.com
  8. ^ Tapu, Codrin Stefan (2001). Hypostatic Personality: Psychopathology of Doing and Being Made. Premier, pp. 18-19. ISBN 9738030595.

Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем сделать НИР

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Mental process — Mental processes, mental functions and cognitive processes are terms often used interchangeably (although not always correctly so, the term cognitive tends to have specific implications – see cognitive and cognitivism) to mean such functions or… …   Wikipedia

  • Mental — Men tal, a. [F., fr. L. mentalis, fr. mens, mentis, the mind; akin to E. mind. See {Mind}.] Of or pertaining to the mind; intellectual; as, mental faculties; mental operations, conditions, or exercise. [1913 Webster] What a mental power This eye… …   The Collaborative International Dictionary of English

  • Mental alienation — Mental Men tal, a. [F., fr. L. mentalis, fr. mens, mentis, the mind; akin to E. mind. See {Mind}.] Of or pertaining to the mind; intellectual; as, mental faculties; mental operations, conditions, or exercise. [1913 Webster] What a mental power… …   The Collaborative International Dictionary of English

  • Mental arithmetic — Mental Men tal, a. [F., fr. L. mentalis, fr. mens, mentis, the mind; akin to E. mind. See {Mind}.] Of or pertaining to the mind; intellectual; as, mental faculties; mental operations, conditions, or exercise. [1913 Webster] What a mental power… …   The Collaborative International Dictionary of English

  • Mental chronometry — For Ian Lowe s book, see Reaction Time (book). Reaction time redirects here. For the biological mechanism, see Reflex. Mental chronometry is the use of response time in perceptual motor tasks to infer the content, duration, and temporal… …   Wikipedia

  • Mental event — A mental event is a particular occurrence of something going on in the mind or mind substitute. It can be a thought, a dream, a feeling, a realization, or any other mental activity. Mental events are not limited to human thought but can be… …   Wikipedia

  • Mental world — The mental world is an ontological category in metaphysics, populated by nonmaterial mental objects, without physical extension (though possibly with mental extension as in a visual field, or possibly not, as in an olfactory field) contrasted… …   Wikipedia

  • mental — mental, ale, aux [ mɑ̃tal, o ] adj. • mentel 1371; bas lat. mentalis, de mens, mentis « esprit » 1 ♦ Qui se fait dans l esprit seulement, sans expression orale ou écrite. Opérations mentales. Calcul mental. Lecture mentale. Restriction mentale. 2 …   Encyclopédie Universelle

  • Mental poker — is the common name for a set of cryptographic problems that concerns playing a fair game over distance without the need for a trusted third party. The term is also applied to the theories surrounding these problems and their possible solutions.… …   Wikipedia

  • Mental calculator — Mental calculators are people with a prodigious ability in some area of mental calculation, such as multiplying large numbers or factoring large numbers. Some rare mental calculators are autistic savants, with a narrow area of great skill and… …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”