Menstrual synchrony

Menstrual synchrony

Menstrual synchrony is a phenomenon reported in 1971 wherein the menstrual cycles of women who lived together (such as in homes, prisons, convents, bordellos, dormitories, or barracks) reportedly became synchronized over time. The existence of menstrual synchrony has not been definitively established, and studies investigating it have been controversial (see also Whitten effect).[citation needed]

Contents

Research

Psychologist Martha McClintock was the first scientist to do a study on menstrual synchrony, reporting her findings in Nature in 1971.[1]

A prospective study by McClintock in 1998 was claimed to support her earlier findings by showing that underarm (or axillary) materials collected from female donors, when wiped under the noses of other women, influenced cycle length.[2] Odors taken on the day that donors ovulated (and the next two days) delayed ovulation and hence lengthened the total cycle of the recipients. These phase-advancing and phase-delaying effects may show human axillary compounds regulate biological rhythms.[3]

Another study experimented with 20 lesbian couples had results which showed that more than half of the couples tested had the same synchronization within a two day period of each other.[4] Another study with 29 lesbian couples showed no evidence of synchrony, and tentative evidence towards divergence of menstrual cycles.[5]

Criticism

If all women had an average-length menstrual cycle (of 28 days duration), the maximum time between two women's onsets would be 14 days and the minimum time between onsets would be zero days (synchronization). On average, the difference would be seven days, and (in small groups) half the time would be less (if one assumes there is no McClintock effect). McClintock observed a five day difference in her 1971 study and some have suggested this could have been a random occurrence.[6]

Most studies of menstrual synchrony have been retrospective, introducing recall bias into the data.[7]

The interaction of theorized menstrual synchrony with differing cycle lengths has not been explained. Two women with cycle lengths that differed by two days might initially begin menstruating on the same day, but the next month would be two days apart, the month after that four days, and so on. No studies have claimed to show that the McClintock effect causes women with historical cycles of different lengths to synchronize.[7]

Methodological errors have also been proposed. A critical review of the evidence for menstrual synchrony gave this example:[7]

Suppose a study starts on October 1. Subject A, with a 28-day cycle, has an onset on September 27, another on October 25, and a third on November 22. Subject B, with a 30-day cycle, has an onset on October 5 and another on November 4. A naive investigator could report that these subjects were 20 days apart at the outset (October 25 vs October 5) and 18 days apart at their second onset (November 4 vs November 22). Therefore, the two are synchronizing. In fact, the two subjects were eight days apart to start with (September 27 vs October 5) and are diverging.

This type of error is more likely in smaller sample sizes, like those used in studies of menstrual synchrony.[7]

H. Clyde Wilson of the University of Missouri analyzed the research and data collection methods McClintock and others used in their studies. He found significant errors in the researchers' mathematical calculations and data collection as well as an error in how the researchers defined synchrony. Wilson's clinical research and his critical reviews of existing research, including the suggestion that pheromones can trigger synchrony in humans,[8] demonstrated that when the studies are corrected for such errors, the evidence for menstrual synchrony disappears.[9]

References

  1. ^ McClintock MK (1971). "Menstrual synchrony and suppression". Nature 229 (5282): 244–5. doi:10.1038/229244a0. PMID 4994256. 
  2. ^ Stern K, McClintock MK (1998). "Regulation of ovulation by human pheromones". Nature 392 (6672): 177–9. doi:10.1038/32408. PMID 9515961. 
  3. ^ Weller, A (1998). "Communication through body odour". Nature 392 (6672): 126–27. doi:10.1038/32283. 
  4. ^ Weller A. Weller L. 1992. menstrual synchrony in female couples. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 17(2-3):171;177
  5. ^ Trevathana, W.R., Burlesonc, M.H. and Gregory, W.L. 1993. No evidence for menstrual synchrony in lesbian couples. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 18(5-6):425;435 [1]
  6. ^ Yang, Zhengwei; Jeffrey C. Schank (2006). "Women Do Not Synchronize Their Menstrual Cycles". Human Nature 17 (4): 434–447. doi:10.1007/s12110-006-1005-z. http://transactionpub.metapress.com/openurl.asp?genre=article&issn=1045-6767&volume=17&issue=4&spage=434. Retrieved 2007-06-25. 
  7. ^ a b c d Adams, Cecil (2002-12-20). "Does menstrual synchrony really exist?". The Straight Dope. The Chicago Reader. http://www.straightdope.com/columns/021220.html. Retrieved 2007-01-10. 
  8. ^ Wilson, H.C. (1987). Female axillary secretions influence women's menstrual cycles: A critique. Hormones and Behavior, 21, 536-546.
  9. ^ Wilson HC (1992). "A critical review of menstrual synchrony research". Psychoneuroendocrinology 17 (6): 565–91. doi:10.1016/0306-4530(92)90016-Z. PMID 1287678. 

External links


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем решить контрольную работу

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Menstrual cycle — See also: Menstruation and Menstruation (mammal) The menstrual cycle is the scientific term for the physiological changes that can occur in fertile women. This article focuses on the human menstrual cycle. The menstrual cycle, under the… …   Wikipedia

  • Menstrual disorder — Classification and external resources ICD 10 N91 N95 ICD 9 …   Wikipedia

  • Menstrual taboo — A Menstrual taboo is any social taboo concerned with menstruation. In some societies it involves menstruation being perceived as unclean or embarrassing, extending even to the mention of menstruation both in public (in the media and advertising)… …   Wikipedia

  • Reproductive synchrony — is a term used in evolutionary biology and behavioural ecology. Reproductive synchrony sometimes termed ovulatory synchrony may manifest itself as breeding seasonality . Where females undergo regular menstruation, menstrual synchrony is another… …   Wikipedia

  • McClintock effect — The McClintock effect, also known as menstrual synchrony or the dormitory effect, is a theory that proposes that the menstrual cycles of women who live together (such as in prisons, convents, bordellos, or dormitories) tend to become synchronized …   Wikipedia

  • Martha McClintock — is an American psychologist best known for her research on human pheromones and her theory of menstrual synchrony. She is the David Lee Shillinglaw Distinguished Service Professor in Psychology at the University of Chicago and is the Founder and… …   Wikipedia

  • Menstruation — Not to be confused with mensuration , a mathematical and a forestry term meaning measurement. See also: Menstrual cycle and Menstruation (mammal) Menstrual cycle Menstruation is the shedding of the uterine lining (endometrium). It occurs on… …   Wikipedia

  • Ovulation — is the process in a female s menstrual cycle by which a mature ovarian follicle ruptures and discharges an ovum (also known as an oocyte, female gamete, or casually, an egg). Ovulation also occurs in the estrous cycle of other female mammals,… …   Wikipedia

  • Dysmenorrhea — Classification and external resources ICD 10 N94.4 N94.6 ICD 9 …   Wikipedia

  • Fertility awareness — Background Birth control type Behavioral First use 1950s (mucus) mid 1930s (BBT) 1930 (Knaus Ogino) Ancient (ad hoc) Failure rates (first year) Perfect use Symptoms based: 1 3% Calendar based: 5 9% …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”