Nuxálk language

Nuxálk language
Nuxálk
Bella Coola
Spoken in Canada
Region Bella Coola area, Central Coast region, British Columbia
Native speakers 20[1]
Language family
Salishan
  • Nuxálk
Language codes
ISO 639-3 blc

Nuxálk (also Bella Coola) is a Salishan language spoken in the vicinity of the Canadian town Bella Coola, British Columbia by approximately 20-30 elders. Until recently, the language was called Bella Coola, but the native designation Nuxálk is now preferred.[2]

Though the number of truly fluent speakers has not increased, the language is now taught in both the provincial school system and the Nuxálk Nation's own school, Acwsalcta, which means "a place of learning". Nuxálk language classes, if taken to at least the Grade 11 level, are considered adequate second language qualifications for entry to the major B.C. universities.

Contents

Name

The name Nuxálk derives from the native nuχalk 'Bella Coola (place)', while Bella Coola derives from Heiltsuk bḷ́xʷlá - "stranger".[3]

Geographical distribution

Nuxálk is spoken in Bella Coola, British Columbia, surrounded by Wakashan- and Athabascan-speaking tribes. It was once spoken in over 100 settlements, with varying dialects, but in the present day most of these settlements have been abandoned and dialectal differences have largely disappeared.[3]

Classification

Nuxálk forms its own subgroup of the Salish language family. Its lexicon is equidistant from Coast and Interior Salish, but it shares phonological and morphological features with Coast Salish (for example, the absence of pharyngeals, and the presence of marked gender). Nuxálk also borrows many words from contiguous North Wakashan languages (especially Heiltsuk), as well as some from neighboring Athabascan languages and Tsimshian.[3]

Sounds

Consonants

The 28 consonants of Nuxálk, with the orthography of (Davis & Saunders 1997, p. 23) when it differs from the IPA. (An alternate orthography without diacritics is shown at the Languagegeek.com link below.):

Bilabial Alveolar Palatal Velar Uvular Glottal
central lateral palatalized labialized plain labialized
Stop aspirated ⟨p⟩ ⟨t⟩ kʲʰ ⟨k⟩ kʷʰ ⟨kʷ⟩ ⟨q⟩ qʷʰ ⟨qʷ⟩ ʔ
ejective ⟨p̓⟩ ⟨t̓⟩ kʼʲ ⟨k̓⟩ kʼʷ ⟨k̓ʷ⟩ ⟨q̓⟩ qʼʷ ⟨q̓ʷ⟩
Affricate aspirated t͡sʰ ⟨c⟩
ejective t͡sʼ ⟨c̓⟩ t͡ɬʼ ⟨ƛ̓⟩
Fricative s ɬ ⟨ł⟩ ⟨x⟩ χ χʷ (h)
Sonorant m n l j ⟨y⟩ w

(Nater 1984, p. 3) postulates the existence of phonemic syllabic consonantal counterparts of the sonorants -- /m̩, n̩, l̩/ (<ṃ ṇ ḷ>), and /i, u/ (reanalysing them as /j,w/).

By this analysis Nuxálk would only have one phonemic vowel, /a/. (Words claimed to have unpredictable syllabics include sṃnṃnṃuuc(ts?) 'mute', smṇmṇc(ts?)aw '(the fact) that they are children'.[4])

Vowels

Front Central Back
Close i u
Open a

Allophony

/i/ may be pronounced:

  • [ɪ] before postvelars
  • [ɪː, ɛː] between postvelars
  • [e̞, e̞ː], before a sonorant followed by a consonant or word boundary
  • [i] adjacent to palatovelars
  • [e] elsewhere

/a/ may be pronounced:

  • [ɑ] ([ɒ]?) surrounded by postvelars
  • [ɐ] before rounded velars followed by a consonant or word boundary
  • [a] ([ä]?) before a sonorant followed by a consonant or word boundary
  • [æ] elsewhere

/u/ may be pronounced:

  • [o̞] surrounded by postvelars
  • [o̞, o̞ː, ɔ, ɔː] before a sonorant followed by a consonant or word boundary
  • [u, ʊ] before rounded velars followed by a consonant or word boundary
  • [o] elsewhere

[5]

Syllables

The notion of syllable is challenged by Nuxálk in that it allows long strings of consonants without any intervening vowel or other sonorant. Salishan languages, and especially Nuxálk, are famous for this. For instance, the following word contains only obstruents:

xłp̓x̣ʷłtłpłłskʷc̓
[xɬpʼχʷɬtʰɬpʰɬːskʷʰt͡sʼ]
'he had had in his possession a bunchberry plant.'
    (Nater 1984, cited in Bagemihl 1991: 16)

Other examples are:

  • [pʰs] 'shape, mold'
  • [pʼs] 'bend'
  • [pʼχʷɬtʰ] 'bunchberry'
  • [t͡sʰkʰtʰskʷʰt͡sʰ] 'he arrived'
  • [tʰt͡sʰ] 'little boy'
  • [skʷʰpʰ] 'saliva'
  • [spʰs] 'northeast wind'
  • [tɬʼpʰ] 'cut with scissors'
  • [st͡sʼqʰ] 'animal fat'
  • [st͡sʼqʰt͡sʰtʰx] 'that's my animal fat over there'
  • [sxs] 'seal fat'
  • [tʰɬ] 'strong'
  • [qʼtʰ] 'go to shore'
  • [qʷʰtʰ] 'crooked'
  • [kʼxɬɬtʰsxʷ sɬχʷtʰɬɬtʰs (t͡s?)] 'you had seen that I had gone through a passage' (Nater 1984, p. 5)

Linguists disagree as to how to count the syllables in such words, what if anything constitutes the nuclei of those syllables, and if the concept of 'syllable' is even applicable to Nuxálk. Some assign every stop consonant in such words to a separate syllable, whereas others attempt to consolidate them. For example, /tɬ/ 'strong' at first appears to be a single syllable with /ɬ/ as the syllable nucleus. However, [tʰt͡sʰ] 'little boy' (phonemically /tt͡s/) may be thought of as having one syllable or two (/t.t͡s/). If one, /t͡s/ would make an unusual nucleus, with /t/ the syllable onset; and if two, both /t/ and /t͡s/ would be considered nuclei, since most theoretical approaches require every syllable to have a nucleus, as part of the definition of 'syllable'. If that assumption is relaxed, so that Nuxálk syllables can be modeled without nuclei, then /tɬ/ 'strong' could be thought of as onset and coda of a single syllable, but it would still not be clear if the /t/ and /t͡s/ of 'little boy' should be considered onset and coda of one syllable, or two onset-only syllables.

Grammar

Events

The first element in a sentence expresses the event of the proposition. It inflects for the person and number of one (in the intransitive paradigm) or two (in the transative paradigm) participants.

Single-participant event inflections[6]
Intr. inflection Singular Plural
First Person -c -(i)ł
Second Person -nu -(n)ap
Third Person -Ø or -s -(n)aw

E.g. ƛ̓ikm-Ø ti-wac̓-tx 'the dog is running'.

Whether the parenthesized segments are included in the suffix depends on whether the stem ends in an underlying resonant (vowel, liquid, nasal) and whether it is non-syllabic. So qāχla 'drink' becomes qāχla-ł 'we drink', qāχla-nap 'you (pl.) drink', qāχla-naw 'they drink', but nuyamł 'sing' becomes nuyamł-ił 'we're singing', nuyamł-ap 'you (pl.) are singing', nuyamł-aw 'they're singing'.

However, the choice of the 3ps marker -Ø or -s is conditioned by semantics rather than phonetics. For example, the sentences tix-s ti-ʔimlk-tx and tix-Ø ti-ʔimlk-tx could both be glossed 'it's the man', but the first is appropriate if the man is the one who is normally chosen, while the second is making an assertion that it is the man (as opposed to someone else, as might otherwise be thought) who is chosen.

The following are the possible person markers for transitive verbs, with empty cells indications non-occurring combinations and '--' identifying semantic combinations which require the reflexive suffix -cut- followed by the appropriate intransitive suffix:

Two-participant event inflections[7]
Transitive
inflection
Experiencer:
Singular Plural
1 2 3 1 2 3
Executor Sg 1 -- -cinu -ic -tułap -tic
2 -cxʷ -- -ixʷ -tułnu -tixʷ
3 -cs -ct -is -tułs -tap -tis
Pl 1 -tułnu -ił -- -tułap -tił
2 -cap -ip -tułp -- -tip
3 -cant -ct -it -tułt -tap -tit

E.g. sp̓-is ti-ʔimlk-tx ti-stn-tx 'the man struck the tree'.

Whether a word can serve as an event isn't determined lexically, e.g. ʔimmllkī-Ø ti-nusʔūlχ-tx 'the thief is a boy', nusʔūlχ-Ø ti-q̓s-tx 'the one who is ill is a thief'.

There is a further causative paradigm whose suffixes may be used instead:

Causitive paradigm[8]
Transitive
inflection
Experiencer:
Singular Plural
1 2 3 1 2 3
Executor Sg 1 -- -tuminu -tuc -tumułap -tutic
2 -tumxʷ -- -tuxʷ -tumułxʷ -tutixʷ
3 -tum -tumt -tus -tumułs -tutap -tutis
Pl 1 -tumułnu -tuł -- -tumułap -tutił
2 -tumanp -tup -tumułp -- -tutip
3 -tumant -tumt -tut -tumułt -tutap -tutit

This has a passive counterpart:

Passive Causative paradigm[9]
Passive Causative Singular Plural
First Person -tuminic -tuminił
Second Person -tumt -tutap
Third Person -tum -tutim

This may also have a benefactive gloss when used with events involving less activity of their participant (e.g. nuyamł-tus ti-ʔimlk-tx ti-ʔimmllkī-tx 'the man made/let the boy sing'/'the man sang for the boy'), while in events will more active participants only the causative gloss is possible. In the later group even more active verbs have a preference for the affix-lx- (implying passive experience) before the causative suffix.

The executor in a transitive sentence always precedes the experiencer. However, when an event is proceeded by a lone participant, the semantic content of the event determines whether the participant is an executor or an experiencer. This can only be determined syntactically if the participant is marked by the preposition ʔuł-, which marks the experience.

Some events are inherently transitive or intransitive, but some may accept multiple valencies (e.g. ʔanayk 'to be needy'/'to want [something]').

Prepositions may mark experiencers, and must mark implements. Any participants which are not marked by prepositions are focussed. There are three voices, which allow either the executor, the experiencer, or both to have focus:

  • Active voice - neither is marked with prepositions.
  • Passive voice - the event may have different suffixes, and the executor may be omitted or marked with a preposition
  • Antipassive voice - the event is marked with the affix -a- before personal markers, and the experiencer is marked with a preposition

The affix -amk- (-yamk- after the antipassive marker -a-) allows an implement to have its preposition removed and to be focused. For example:

  • nuyamł-Ø ti-man-tx ʔuł-ti-mna-s-tx x-ti-syut-tx 'the father sang the song to his son'
  • nuyamł-amk-is ti-man-tx ti-syut-tx ʔuł-ti-mna-s-tx 'the father sang the song to his son'

Prepositions

There are four prepositions which have broad usage in Nuxálk:

Prepositions[10]
Prepositions Proximal Distal
Stative x- ʔał-
Active ʔuł- wixłł-

Deixis

Nuxálk has a set of deictic prefixes and suffixes which serve to identify items as instantiations of domains rather than domains themselves and to locate them in deictic space. Thus the sentences wac̓-Ø ti-ƛ̓ikm-tx and ti-wac̓-Ø ti-ƛ̓ikm-tx, both 'the one that's running is a dog', are slightly different - similar to the difference between the English sentences 'the visitor is Canadian' and 'the visitor is a Canadian' respectively.[11]

The deixis system has a proximal/medial/distal and a non-demonstrative/demonstrative distinction. Demonstratives may be used when finger pointing would be appropriate (or in distal space when something previously mentioned is being referred to).

Proximal demonstrative space roughly corresponds to the area of conversation, and proximal non-demonstrative may be viewed as the area in which one could attract another's attention without raising one's voice. Visible space beyond this is middle demonstrative, space outside of this but within the invisible neighborhood is medial non-demonstrative. Everything else is distal, and non-demonstrative if not mentioned earlier.

The deictic prefixes and suffixes are as follows:

Deictic suffixes[12]
Deictic
Suffixes
Proximal Medial Distal
Non-Demon-
strative
Demon-
strative
Non-Demon-
strative
Demon-
strative
Non-Demon-
strative
Demon-
strative
Masculine -tx -t̓ayx -t̓aχ -tχ -taχ
Feminine -cx -c̓ayx -ʔiłʔaył -ʔił -ʔił
Plural -c -ʔac -t̓aχʷ -tχʷ -tuχ

Female affixes are used only when the particular is singular and identified as female; if not, even if the particular is inanimate, masculine or plural is used.

The deictic suffixes only have a proximal vs. non-proximal distinction, and no demonstrative distinction:

Deictic prefixes[13]
Deictic
Prefixes
Proximal Medial and Distal
Masculine ti- ta-
Feminine ci- ła- (ʔił-)
Plural wa- ta- (tu-)

tu- is used in earlier varieties and some types of narratives, except for middle non-demonstrative, and the variant ʔił- may be used "in the same collection of deictic space".

While events are not explicitely marked for tense per se, deixis plays a strong role in determining when the proposition is being asserted to occur. So in a sentence like mus-is ti-ʔimmllkī-tx ta-q̓lsxʷ-t̓aχ 'the boy felt that rope', the sentence is perceived as having a near-past (same day) interpretation, as the boy cannot be touching the rope in middle space from proximal space. However this does not hold for some events, like k̓x 'to see'.[14]

A distal suffix on any participant lends the event a distant past interpretation (before the past day), a medial suffix and no distal suffix lends a near past time, and if the participants are marked as proximal the time is present.

Not every distal participant occurs in past-tense sentences, and vice versa—rather, the deictic suffixes must either represent positions in space, time, or both.

The "/-m/" suffix of the Bella Coola language is one of the most puzzling verbal affixes in the language. Some argue that it has varying uses of its morpheme, or that the suffix itself represents different morphemes due to the transitive bases the suffix consists of. The plural of the "/-m/" suffix has no known cognates. Another suffix of the Bella Coola language is the "/-uks/" suffix. This suffix was never recorded, and the its derivatives are skeptical. Some say that, because of the /-uks/ suffix, Bella Coola influenced the Wakashan and Athapaskan languages, also originating from the British Colombian coast. Others believe, though, that the /-uks/ language used in the Bella Coola language were previously recorded in the Chinook Jargon, thus it was taken from that language. At this point in time, linguists have two stances on this argument: either /-uks/ did originate from the Chinook jargon, or /-uks/ is one of the few elements originating from languages spoken south of the Salishan area of the British Colombia coast, which is difficult to decipher due to the lack of recorded evidence on it. Linguists are unsure what this meaning could bring. An opinion sometimes considered is that people of all the mentioned languages, from Chinook to Bella Coola to Wakashan/Athapaskan, were somehow congregated together, and its people were, for more than a brief amount of time, associated with one another. This could have derived from inter-tribal marriages, which meshed the different structural components of the language to form one unique, syntactical language structure. We cannot testify to this hypothesis, though, due to the lack of archives previously produced/left behind by the people that once spoke these languages fluently.

Pronouns

Pronouns[15] Singular Plural
Speaker ʔnc łmił
Listener ʔinu łup
Non-speaker/
listener
tix,cix wix

Particles

Particles[16]
Particle Label Gloss
Quotative 'he said'
ma Dubitative 'maybe'
ʔalu Attemptive 'try'
ck Inferential Dubitative 'I figure'
cakʷ Optative 'I wish/hope'
su Expectable 'again'
tu Confirmative 'really'
ku Surprisative 'so'
lu Expective 'expected'
a Interrogative [yes/no questions]
Perfective 'now'
c̓n Imperfective 'now'
k̓ʷ Usitative 'usually'
mas Absolutive 'always'
ks Individuative 'the one'
łū Persistive 'still, yet'
Non-contrastive
conjunction
'and'
ʔi...k Contrastive
conjunction
'but'

See also

References

  1. ^ Ethnologue
  2. ^ Suttles, Wayne (1990). "Introduction". In "Northwest Coast", ed. Wayne Suttles. Vol. 7 of Handbook of North American Indians, ed. William C. Sturtevant, pg. 15
  3. ^ a b c Nater 1984, p. xvii
  4. ^ Nater 1984, p. 14
  5. ^ Nater 1984, p. 5
  6. ^ Davis & Saunders 1997, p. 24.
  7. ^ Davis & Saunders 1997, p. 26.
  8. ^ Davis & Saunders 1997, p. 29.
  9. ^ Davis & Saunders 1997, p. 43.
  10. ^ Davis & Saunders 1997, p. 36.
  11. ^ Davis & Saunders 1997, pp. 83-84.
  12. ^ Davis & Saunders 1997, p. 86.
  13. ^ Davis & Saunders 1997, p. 89.
  14. ^ Davis & Saunders 1997, pp. 89-90.
  15. ^ Davis & Saunders 1997, p. 114.
  16. ^ Davis & Saunders, p. 180.

Bibliography

  • Bruce Bagemihl (1991). "Syllable structure in Bella Coola". Proceedings of the New England Linguistics Society 21: 16–30. 
  • Bruce Bagemihl (1991). "Syllable structure in Bella Coola". Linguistic Inquiry 22: 589–646. 
  • Bruce Bagemihl (1998). Maximality in Bella Coola (Nuxalk). In E. Czaykowska-Higgins & M. D. Kinkade (Eds.), Salish languages and linguistics: Theoretical and descriptive perspectives (pp. 71–98). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
  • Philip W. Davis & Ross Saunders (1973). "Lexical suffix copying in Bella Coola". Glossa 7: 231–252. 
  • Philip W. Davis & Ross Saunders (1975). "Bella Coola nominal deixis". Language (Language, Vol. 51, No. 4) 51 (4): 845–858. doi:10.2307/412696. JSTOR 412696. 
  • Philip W. Davis & Ross Saunders (1976). "Bella Coola deictic roots". International Journal of American Linguistics 42 (4): 319–330. doi:10.1086/465436. 
  • Philip W. Davis & Ross Saunders (1978). Bella Coola syntax. In E.-D. Cook & J. Kaye (Eds.), Linguistic studies of native Canada (pp. 37–66). Vancouver: University of British Columbia.
  • Philip W. Davis & Ross Saunders (1979). "Bella Coola phonology". Lingua 49 (2–3): 169–187. doi:10.1016/0024-3841(79)90022-6. 
  • Philip W. Davis & Ross Saunders (1980). Bella Coola texts. British Columbia Provincial Museum heritage record (No. 10). Victoria: British Columbia Provincial Museum. ISBN 0-7718-8206-8.
  • Philip W. Davis & Ross Saunders (1997). A grammar of Bella Coola. University of Montana occasional papers in linguistics (No. 13). Missoula, MT: University of Montana. ISBN 1-8797-6313-3.
  • Forrest, Linda. (1994). The de-transitive clause in Bella Coola: Passive vs inverse. In T. Givón (Ed.), Voice and inversion (pp. 147–168). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
  • Mithun, Marianne. (1999). The languages of Native North America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-23228-7 (hbk); ISBN 0-521-29875-X.
  • Montler, Timothy. (2004–2005). (Handouts on Salishan language family).
  • Nater, Hank F. (1977). Stem list of the Bella Coola language. Lisse: Peter de Ridder.
  • Nater, Hank F. (1984). The Bella Coola language. Mercury series; Canadian ethonology service (No. 92). Ottawa: National Museums of Canada.
  • Nater, Hank F. (1990). A concise Nuxalk-English dictionary. Mercury series; Canadian ethonology service (No. 115). Hull, Quebec: Canadian Museum of Civilization. ISBN 0-6601-0798-8.
  • Newman, Stanley. (1947). Bella Coola I: Phonology. International Journal of American Linguistics, 13, 129-134.
  • Newman, Stanley. (1969). Bella Coola grammatical processes and form classes. International Journal of American Linguistics, 35, 175-179.
  • Newman, Stanley. (1969). Bella Coola paradigms. International Journal of American Linguistics, 37, 299-306.
  • Newman, Stanley. (1971). Bella Coola reduplication. International Journal of American Linguistics, 37, 34-38.
  • Newman, Stanley. (1974). Language retention and diffusion in Bella Coola. Language in Society, 3, 201-214.
  • Newman, Stanley. (1976). Salish and Bella Coola prefixes. International Journal of American Linguistics, 42, 228-242.
  • Newman, Stanley. (1989). Lexical morphemes in Bella Coola. In M. R. Key & H. Hoenigswald (Eds.), General and Amerindian ethnolinguistics: In remembrance of Stanley Newman (pp. 289–301). Contributions to the sociology of language (No. 55). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. ISBN 0-8992-5519-1.

External links


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Нужно решить контрольную?

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Nuxalk — Nuxálk (also Bella Coola) are an indigenous people native to Bella Coola, British Columbia in Canada. The term can refer to: Nuxálk language, a moribund Salishan language. Nuxalk Nation, the name of the Nuxálk group in the First Nations. This… …   Wikipedia

  • Nuxalk Nation — Bella Coola (Nuxalk Nation) Nuxálk Nuxálk people gathered around an eulachon stink box near the Bella Coola River. Regions with significant populations Bella Coola, British Columbia …   Wikipedia

  • Carrier language — Carrier Dakeł (ᑕᗸᒡ) Spoken in Canada Region Central Interior of British Columbia Ethnicity Carrier people Nati …   Wikipedia

  • Oowekyala language — Oowekyala (also Wuikala, Rivers Inlet, Wuikenukv, Oweekeno, Wikeno, Owikeno, Oowekeeno, Oweekano, Awikenox, Oowek yala, Oweek ala) is a dialect (or a sublanguage) of Heiltsuk Oowekyala, a Northern Wakashan (Kwakiutlan) language spoken around… …   Wikipedia

  • Native countries of North America — The following is a list of names of Native countries of North America the homelands of American Indian and Inuitic peoples. Autonymic names in Native American languages are in bold, translations of the Native names are in parentheses and quotes.… …   Wikipedia

  • King Island (British Columbia) — King Island is an island on the coast of the Canadian province of British Columbia. It is located south of Dean Channel and about convert|20|km|mi east of Bella Bella, and within the Central Coast Regional District. A number of other islands… …   Wikipedia

  • Bella Coola — may refer to several things, all closely related to a geographic area within British Columbia s Central Coast:*The Nuxálk, an indigenous people of the area who in the past had been referred to as the Bella Coola *The Nuxálk language spoken by… …   Wikipedia

  • Tallheo, British Columbia — Tallheo is a First Nations community of the Nuxálk people and former cannery town near Bella Coola, British Columbia, Canada, on North Bentinck Arm. Tallheo is also the name of the dialect of the Nuxálk language spoken by the Talhyumc, the… …   Wikipedia

  • Indigenous peoples of the Pacific Northwest Coast — This article is about the indigenous peoples of the Pacific Northwest Coast. For other indigenous peoples see Indigenous peoples (disambiguation) Chief Anotklosh of the Taku Tribe of the Tlingit people, ca. 1913 The Indigenous peoples of the… …   Wikipedia

  • List of traditional territories of the indigenous peoples of North America — The following is a list of names of the territories of indigenous peoples of the North American continent. Autonymic names in native North American languages are in bold, translations of the Native names are in parentheses and quotes. The Native… …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”