Copycat suicide

Copycat suicide

A copycat suicide is defined as an emulation of another suicide that the person attempting suicide knows about either from local knowledge or due to accounts or depictions of the original suicide on television and in other media.

The massive wave of emulation suicides after a widely publicized suicide is known as the Werther effect, following Goethe's novel The Sorrows of Young Werther.[1]

The well-known suicide serves as a model, in the absence of protective factors, for the next suicide. This is referred to as suicide contagion.[2] They occasionally spread through a school system, through a community, or in terms of a celebrity suicide wave, nationally. This is called a suicide cluster.[2] Suicide clusters are caused by the social learning of suicide related behaviors, or "copycat suicides". Point clusters are clusters of suicides in both time and space, and have been linked to direct social learning from nearby individuals.[3] Mass clusters are clusters of suicides in time but not space, and have been linked to the broadcasting of information concerning celebrity suicides via the mass media[4] Examples of celebrities whose suicides have inspired suicide clusters include the Japanese musicians Yukiko Okada and Hide and Marilyn Monroe, whose death was followed by an increase of 200 more suicides than average for that August month.[2]

Another famous case is the self-immolation of Mohamed Bouazizi, a Tunisian street vendor who set himself on fire on December 17, 2010, an act that was a catalyst for Tunisian Revolution and sparked protests in several other Arab countries, including several men who emulated Bouazizi's act.

To prevent this type of suicide, it is customary in some countries for the media to discourage suicide reports except in special cases.

Contents

History

One of the earliest known associations between the media and suicide arose from Goethe's novel Die Leiden des jungen Werthers (The Sorrows of Young Werther). Goethe's novel was published in 1774 and not long after young men began to mimic the character Werther by dressing in yellow pants and blue jackets. The new fashion trend seemed to be entertaining to the public. A new trend also emerged from the book causing it to be banned in numerous areas. In the novel, Werther commits suicide with a pistol after he fails to get the girl he desires. Many men replicated this trend in an act of hopelessness.[5] In that work the hero shoots himself after an ill-fated love, and shortly after its publication there were many reports of young men using the same method to commit suicide. This resulted in a ban of the book in several places. Hence the term "Werther effect", used in the technical literature to designate copycat suicides.[6] The term was coined by researcher David Phillips in 1974.[7] Two centuries after Goethe’s novel was published David Phillips confirmed imitative suicides as the “Werther effect.” Reports in 1985 and 1989 by Phillips and his colleagues found that suicides and other accidents seem to incline after a well publicized suicide.[5] Copycat suicide is mostly blamed on the media. "Hearing about a suicide seems to make those who are vulnerable feel they have permission to do it," Dr. Phillips said. He cited studies that showed that people were more likely to engage in dangerous deviant behavior, such as drug taking, if someone else had set the example first.[3]

Factors in suicide reporting

The Werther effect not only predicts an increase in suicide, but the majority of the suicides will take place in the same or a similar way as the one publicized. The more similar the person in the publicized suicide is to the people exposed to the information about it, the more likely the age group or demographic is to commit suicide. The increase generally happens only in areas where the suicide story was highly publicized.[5] Upon learning of someone else's suicide, many people decide that action is appropriate for them as well, especially if the publicized suicide was of someone in a similar situation as them.

Publishing the means of suicides, romanticized and sensationalized reporting, particularly about celebrities, suggestions that there is an epidemic, glorifying the deceased and simplifying the reasons all lead to increases in the suicide rate. People may see suicide as a glamorous ending — with youth getting a lot of attention, lots of sympathy, lots of national concern that they never got in life (Guard, Anara). The second possible factor is that vulnerable youth may feel like, "If they couldn't cut it, neither can I".[8] Increased rate of suicides has been shown to occur up to ten days after a television report.[9] Studies in Japan[10] and Germany[11] have replicated findings of an imitative effect. Etzersdorfer et al.[12] in an Austrian study showed a strong correlation between the number of papers distributed in various areas and the number of subsequent firearm suicides in each area after a related media report. Higher rates of copycat suicides have been found in those with similarities in race,[10] age, and gender[13] to the victim in the original report. Stack[14] analyzed the results from 42 studies and found that those measuring the effect of a celebrity suicide story were 14.3 times more likely to find a copycat effect than studies that did not. Studies based on a real as opposed to fictional story were 4.03 times more likely to uncover a copycat effect and research based on televised stories was 82% less likely to report a copycat effect than research based on newspapers. Other scholars have been less certain about whether copycat suicides truly happen or are selectively hyped. For instance, fears of a suicide wave following the death of Kurt Cobain never materialized in an actual increase in suicides.[15] Furthermore, there is evidence for an indirect Werther effect, i.e. the perception that suicidal media content has an impact on others which, in turn, can concurrently or additionally influence one person´s own future thoughts and behaviors.[16] Similarly the researcher Gerard Sullivan has critiqued research on copycat suicides, suggesting that data analyses have been selective and misleading, and that the evidence for copycat suicides are much less consistent than suggested by some researchers.[17]

The suicide of Mohamed Bouazizi, a Tunisian street vendor who set himself on fire on December 17, 2010, in protest of the confiscation of his wares and the harassment and humiliation that was allegedly inflicted on him by a municipal official and her aides, became the catalyst for the Tunisian Revolution, that culminated with the end of then-President Zine El Abidine Ben Ali 23 year old regime. The success of the Tunisian protests sparked protests in several other Arab countries, including several men who emulated Bouazizi's act, in an attempt to bring an end to autocratic governments. Those men and Bouazizi are hailed by some Arab commentators as "heroic martyrs of a new Middle Eastern revolution".[18]

Many people interviewed after the suicide of a relative or friend have a tendency to simplify the issues; their grief can lead to their minimizing or ignoring significant factors. Studies show a high incidence of psychiatric disorders in suicide victims at the time of their death with the total figure ranging from 98%[19] to 87.3%[20] with mood disorders and substance abuse being the two most common. These are often undiagnosed or untreated and treatment can result in reductions in the suicide rate. Reports that minimize the impact of psychiatric disorders contribute to copycat suicides whereas reports that mention this factor and provide help-line contact numbers and advice for where sufferers may gain assistance can reduce suicides.

Social proof model

An alternate model to explain copycat suicide, called "social proof" by Cialdini,[21] goes beyond the theories of glorification and simplification of reasons to look at why copycat suicides are so similar, demographically and in actual methods, to the original publicized suicide. In the social proof model, people imitate those who seem similar, despite or even because of societal disapproval. This model is important because it has nearly opposite ramifications for what the media ought to do about the copycat suicide effect than the standard model does.[citation needed] To deal with this problem, Alex Mesoudi of Queen Mary University, London, developed a computer model of a community of 1000 people, to examine how copycat suicides occur. These were divided into 100 groups of 10, in a model designed to represent different levels of social organization, such as schools or hospitals within a town or state. Mesoudi then circulated the simulation through 100 generations. He found the simulated people acted just as sociologists' theory predicted.They were more likely to commit suicide in clusters, either because they had learned this trait from their friends, or because suicidal people are more likely to be like one another.[22]

Journalism codes

Various countries have national journalism codes which range from one extreme of, "Suicide and attempted suicide should in general never be given any mention" (Norway) to a more moderate, "In cases of suicide, publishing or broadcasting information in an exaggerated way that goes beyond normal dimensions of reporting with the purpose of influencing readers or spectators should not occur. The study's author, University of London psychologist Alex Mesoudi, recommends reporters to follow the sort of guidelines the World Health Organization and others endorse for coverage of any suicide: Use extreme restraint in covering these deaths — keep the word "suicide" out of the headline, don't romanticize the death, and limit the number of stories.[23] Photography, pictures, visual images or film depicting such cases should not be made public" (Turkey).[24] Many countries do not have national codes but do have in-house guidelines along similar lines. In the US there are no industrywide standards and a survey of inhouse guides of 16 US daily newspapers showed that only three mentioned the word suicide and none gave guidelines about publishing the method of suicide. Craig Branson, online director of the American Society of News Editors (ASNE), has been quoted as saying, "Industry codes are very generic and totally voluntary. Most ethical decisions are left to individual editors at individual papers. The industry would fight any attempt to create more specific rules or standards, and editors would no doubt ignore them."[24] Guidelines on the reporting of suicides in Ireland were introduced recently which attempt to remove any positive connotations the act might have (e.g. using the term "completed" rather than "successful" when describing a suicide attempt which resulted in a death).[citation needed]

The Canadian Broadcasting Corporation's journalistic standards and practices manual discourages the reporting of the details of suicide. [25]

Journalist training

Australia is one of the few countries where there is a concerted effort to teach journalism students about this subject. The Mindframe national media initiative[26] followed an ambivalent response by the Australian Press Council to an earlier media resource kit issued by Suicide Prevention Australia and the Australian Institute for Suicide Research and Prevention. The UK-based media ethics charity MediaWise provides training for journalists on reporting suicide related issues.

Headline is Ireland's media monitoring programme for suicide and mental health issues, set up by Shine and the Health Service Executives National Office for Suicide Prevention as part of 'Reach Out: National Strategy for action on Suicide Prevention.' Headline works with media professionals and students to find ways to collaborate to ensure that suicide, mental health and mental illness are responsibly covered in the media and provides information on reporting on mental health and suicidal behavior, literature and daily analysis of news stories. Headline also serves as a vehicle for the public to become involved in helping to monitor the Irish media on issues relating to mental health and suicide.

See also

In art

References

  1. ^ The Werther effect after television films: new evidence for an old hypothesis, Psychol Med. 1988 Aug;18(3):665-76.
  2. ^ a b c Halgin, Richard P.; Susan Whitbourne (January 2006). Abnormal Psychology with MindMap II CD-ROM and PowerWeb. McGraw-Hill. pp. 62. ISBN 0-07-322872-9. 
  3. ^ a b Golman, D. "Pattern of Death: Copycat Suicides among Youths.". 1987. New York Times. http://www.nytimes.com/1987/03/18/nyregion/pattern-of-death-copycat-suicides-among-youths.html. Retrieved April 21, 2011. 
  4. ^ Mesoudi, A (2009). "The Cultural Dynamics of Copycat Suicide.". PLoS ONE 4 (9). 
  5. ^ a b c Meyers, David G. (2009). Social Psychology (10th Ed). New York: McGraw Hill. ISBN 978-0-07-337066-8. 
  6. ^ "Preventing suicide: A report for media professionals" (PDF). World Health Organization. 2000. http://www.who.int/entity/mental_health/media/en/426.pdf. Retrieved 2007-06-09. 
  7. ^ De Wyze, Jeannette (2005-02-31). "Why Do They Die?". San Diego Reader. http://www.sandiegoreader.com/news/2005/mar/31/why-do-they-die/. Retrieved 2008-02-05. 
  8. ^ Mulvihill, G (2010). "Experts Fear Copycat Suicides After Bullying Cases. Experts fear a spate of copycat suicides after bullying cases grab national headlines.". ABC News. http://abcnews.go.com/Health/wireStory?id=12122386. Retrieved April 21, 2011. 
  9. ^ Phillips, David P. (May 1982). "The Impact of Fictional Television Stories on U.S. Adult Fatalities: New Evidence on the Effect of the Mass Media on Violence". The American Journal of Sociology 87 (6): 1340–59. doi:10.1086/227596. 
  10. ^ a b Stack S. (1996) The effect of the media on suicide: evidence from Japan, 1955-1985. Suicide Life Threat Behav., 26 (2) :132-42. PMID 8840417
  11. ^ Jonas K. (1992) Modelling and suicide: a test of the Werther effect. Br J Soc Psychol., Dec;31 (Pt 4):295-306. PMID 1472984
  12. ^ Etzersdorfer E, Voracek M, Sonneck G. (2004) A dose-response relationship between imitational suicides and newspaper distribution. Arch Suicide Res., 8(2):137-45. PMID 16006399
  13. ^ Schmidtke A, Häfner H. (1988) The Werther effect after television films: new evidence for an old hypothesis. Psychol Med., Aug;18(3):665-76. PMID 3263660
  14. ^ Stack S (2002). "Media coverage as a risk factor in suicide". Inj. Prev. 8 Suppl 4: IV30–2. PMC 1765497. PMID 12460954. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=1765497. 
  15. ^ Jobes, D., Berman, A., O'Carroll, P., & Eastgard, S. (1996). The Kurt Cobain suicide crisis: Perspectives from research, public health and the news media. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 26(3), 260-271.
  16. ^ Scherr, S., & Reinemann, C. (in press). Belief in a Werther effect. Third-Person effects in the perceptions of suicide risk for others and the moderating role of depression. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior (Online First).
  17. ^ Sullivan, G. (2007). Should Suicide Be Reported in the Media? A Critique of Research. Remember me: Constructing immortality--Beliefs on immortality, life and death (pp. 149-158). New York, NY US: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
  18. ^ Worth, Robert F. (2011-01-21). "How a Single Match Can Ignite a Revolution". New York Times. http://www.nytimes.com/2011/01/23/weekinreview/23worth.html?src=twrhp. Retrieved 2011-01-26. 
  19. ^ Bertolote JM, Fleischmann A, De Leo D, Wasserman D. (2004) Psychiatric diagnoses and suicide: revisiting the evidence. Crisis., 25(4):147-55. doi:10.1027/0227-5910.25.4.147 PMID 15580849
  20. ^ Arsenault-Lapierre G, Kim C, Turecki G. (2004) Psychiatric diagnoses in 3275 suicides: a meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry, Nov 4;4:37. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-4-37 PMID 15527502
  21. ^ Robert B. Cialdini (1993). Influence: the psychology of persuasion. New York: Morrow. p. 336. ISBN 0-688-12816-5. 
  22. ^ Hamzelou, J (2009). "Copycat suicides fuelled by media reports.". Newscientist. http://www.newscientist.com/article/dn17889-copycat-suicides-fuelled-by-media-reports.html. Retrieved April 21, 2011. 
  23. ^ Franklin, D (2009). "Copycat Suicides: What's The Media's Role?". NPR. http://www.npr.org/blogs/health/2009/09/copycat_suicides_whats_the_med.html. Retrieved April 21, 2011. 
  24. ^ a b Norris, Bill; Mike Jempson; Lesley Bygrave (September 2001). "Covering suicide worldwide: media responsibilities" (PDF). The MediaWise Trust. Archived from the original on 2007-06-14. http://web.archive.org/web/20070614001050/http://www.mediawise.org.uk/files/uploaded/Covering+Suicide+Worldwide.pdf. Retrieved 2007-06-09. 
  25. ^ "Suicide" (HTML). Journalistic standards and practices. CBC/Radio-Canada. http://www.cbc.radio-canada.ca/docs/policies/journalistic/xml/policies.asp?pol=138_en.xml. Retrieved 2011-10-17. 
  26. ^ "Reporting Suicide: Guidance for journalists". The MediaWise Trust, Spanish, French. http://www.mediawise.org.uk/display_page.php?id=171. Retrieved 2007-06-09. 

Further reading

External links


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем написать курсовую

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Copycat — may refer to: Copycat (Marvel Comics), a comic book character in the Marvel Universe Copycat (Wildstorm), a comic book character in the Wildstorm Comics Universe Copycat (film), a 1995 thriller starring Sigourney Weaver and Holly Hunter Copycat… …   Wikipedia

  • Copycat effect — may refer to: Copycat crimes crimes inspired by or replicating another crime Copycat suicide suicide inspired by or replicating another s suicide This disambiguation page lists articles associated with the same title. If an …   Wikipedia

  • Copycat — Cette page d’homonymie répertorie les différents sujets et articles partageant un même nom. Un copycat est une personne agissant de manière similaire à une action lue dans les médias. Il peut s agir de tuer suivant le modus operandi d un serial… …   Wikipédia en Français

  • Suicide — For other uses, see Suicide (disambiguation). Suicide Classification and external resources …   Wikipedia

  • Copycat crime — A copycat crime is a criminal act that is modelled or inspired by a previous crime that has been reported in the media or described in fiction. Contents 1 Copycat effect 2 Causation 3 Statistics 4 See also …   Wikipedia

  • Suicide note — A suicide note is a message left by someone who later attempts or commits suicide. It is estimated that 12 20% of suicides are accompanied by a note. [cite web|url=http://www.aim.org/aim report/A3630 0 4 0 C/|title=AIM Report: Critiquing Berman s …   Wikipedia

  • Suicide by cop — Suicide Social …   Wikipedia

  • Suicide intervention — is a direct effort to prevent person(s) from attempting to take ones own life intentionally or voluntary through self destructive behaviour such as; hanging ones self, cutting wrists, (Webster, n.d). “Suicide rates vary throughout the world, but… …   Wikipedia

  • Suicide by hanging — Suicide Social …   Wikipedia

  • Suicide pact — Suicide Social …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”