Fitts's law

Fitts's law

In human-computer interaction and ergonomics, Fitts's law (often cited as Fitts' law) is a model of human movement which predicts the time required to rapidly move to a target area, as a function of the distance to the target and the size of the target.

Fitts's law is used to model the act of "pointing", both in the real world (e.g., with a hand or finger) and on computers (e.g., with a mouse). It was published by Paul Fitts in 1954.

The model

Mathematically, Fitts's law has been formulated in several different ways. One common form is the Shannon formulation (proposed by Scott MacKenzie, professor of York University, and named for its resemblance to the Shannon-Hartley theorem) for movement along a single dimension:

: T = a + b log_2 Bigg(frac{D}{W}+1Bigg)

where

* "T" is the average time taken to complete the movement. (Traditionally, researchers have used the symbol "MT" for this, to mean "movement time".)
* "a" represents the start/stop time of the device and "b" stands for the inherent speed of the device. These constants can be determined experimentally by fitting a straight line to measured data.
* "D" is the distance from the starting point to the center of the target. (Traditionally, researchers have used the symbol "A" for this, to mean the "amplitude" of the movement.)
* "W" is the width of the target measured along the axis of motion. "W" can also be thought of as the allowed error tolerance in the final position, since the final point of the motion must fall within ± "W"/2 of the target's centre.

From the equation, we see a "speed-accuracy" tradeoff associated with pointing, whereby targets that are smaller and/or further away require more time to acquire.

Success and implications

Fitts's law is an unusually successful and well-studied model. Experiments that reproduce Fitts's results and/or that demonstrate the applicability of Fitts's law in somewhat different situations are not difficult to perform. The measured data in such experiments often fit a straight line with a correlation coefficient of 0.95 or higher, a sign that the model is very accurate.

Although Fitts only published two articles on his law (Fitts 1954, Fitts and Peterson 1964), there are hundreds of subsequent studies related to it in the human-computer interaction (HCI) literature, and quite possibly thousands of studies published in the larger psychomovement literature. The first HCI application of Fitts's law was by Card, English, and Burr (1978), who used the index of performance ("IP"), defined as 1/"b", to compare performance of different input devices, with the mouse coming out on top. (This early work, according to Stuart Card's biography, "was a major factor leading to the mouse's commercial introduction by Xerox" [http://www.bcs-hci.org.uk/interfaces/interfaces50.pdf] .) Fitts's law has been shown to apply under a variety of conditions, with many different limbs (hands, feet, head-mounted sights, eye gaze), manipulanda (input devices), physical environments (including underwater), and user populations (young, old, special educational needs, and drugged participants).Note that the constants "a", "b", "IP" have different values under each of these conditions.

Since the advent of graphical user interfaces, Fitts's law has been applied to tasks where the user must position a mouse cursor over an on-screen target, such as a button or other widget. Fitts's law models both point-and-click and drag-and-drop actions. Dragging has a lower "IP" associated with it, because the increased muscle tension makes pointing more difficult.

In its original and strictest form:

* It applies only to movement in a single dimension and not to movement in two dimensions (though it is successfully extended to two dimensions in the Accot-Zhai steering law);
* It describes simple motor response of, say, the human hand, failing to account for software acceleration usually implemented for a mouse cursor;
* It describes untrained movements, not movements that are executed after months or years of practice (though some argue that Fitts's law models behaviour that is so low level that extensive training doesn't make much difference).

If, as generally claimed, the law does hold true for pointing with the mouse, some consequences for user-interface design includeFact|date=May 2007:

* Buttons and other GUI controls should be a reasonable size; it is relatively difficult to click on small ones.
* Edges (e.g. the menubar at top and Dock at bottom in Mac OS X) and corners of the computer display (e.g. "Start" button in the Luna theme of Windows XP and Apple & Spotlight menus of Mac OS X) are particularly easy to acquire because the pointer remains at the screen edge regardless of how much further the mouse is moved, thus can be considered as having infinite width.
* Pop-up menus can usually be opened faster than pull-down menus, since the user avoids travel.
* Pie menu items typically are selected faster and have a lower error rate than linear menu items, for two reasons: because pie menu items are all the same, small distance from the centre of the menu; and because their wedge-shaped target areas (which usually extend to the edge of the screen) are very large.

Fitts's law remains one of the few hard, reliable human-computer interaction predictive models, joined more recently by the Accot-Zhai steering law, which is derived from Fitts's law.

Mathematical details

The logarithm in Fitts's law is called the index of difficulty "ID" for the target, and has units of bits. We can rewrite the law as

: T = a + b ID,,

where

: ID = log_2 left(frac{D}{W}+1 ight)

Thus, the units for "b" are time/bit, e.g. milliseconds/bit. The constant "a" can be thought of as incorporating reaction time and/or the time required to click a button.

The values for "a" and "b" change as the conditions under which pointing is done are changed. For example, a mouse and stylus may both be used for pointing, but have different constants "a" and "b" associated with them.

An index of performance "IP" (also called throughput "TP"), in bits/time, can be defined to characterize how quickly pointing can be done, independent of the particular targets involved. There are two conventions for defining "IP": one is "IP" = 1/"b" (which has the disadvantage of ignoring the effect of "a"), the other is "IP" = "ID"average/"MT"average (which has the disadvantage of depending on an arbitrarily chosen "average" "ID"). For a discussion of these two conventions, see Zhai (2002). Whatever definition is used, measuring the "IP" of different input devices allows the devices to be compared with respect to their pointing capability.

Slightly different from the Shannon formulation is the original formulation by Fitts:

: ID = log_2 left(frac{2D}{W} ight)

The factor of 2 here is not particularly important; this form of the "ID" can be rewritten with the factor of 2 absorbed as changes in the constants "a", "b". The "+1" in the Shannon form, however, does make it different from Fitts's original form, especially for low values of the ratio "D"/"W". The Shannon form has the advantage that the "ID" is always non-negative, and has been shown to better fit measured data.

Derivation

Fitts's law can be derived from various models of motion. A very simple model, involving discrete, deterministic responses, is considered here. Although this model is overly simplistic, it provides some intuition for Fitts's law.

Assume that the user moves toward the target in a sequence of submovements. Each submovement requires a constant time "t" to execute, and moves a constant fraction 1-"r" of the remaining distance to the centre of the target, where 0 < "r" < 1. Thus, if the user is initially at a distance "D" from the target, the remaining distance after the first submovement is "rD", and the remaining distance after the "n"th submovement is "rnD". (In other words, the distance left to the target's centre is a function that decays exponentially over time.) Let "N" be the (possibly fractional) number of submovements required to fall within the target. Then,

: r^N D = frac{W}{2}

Solving for "N":

: egin{align}N & = log_r frac{W}{2D} \& = frac{1}{log_2 r} log_2 frac{W}{2D}quad( ext{since } log_x y = (log_z y)/(log_z x)) \& = frac{1}{log_2 1/r} log_2 frac{2D}{W}quad( ext{since } log_x y = - log_x(1/y)).end{align}

The time required for all submovements is:

: egin{align}T = Nt & = frac{t}{log_2 1/r} log_2 frac{2D}{W} \& = frac{t}{log_2 1/r} + frac{t}{log_2 1/r} log_2 frac{D}{W}.end{align}

By defining appropriate constants "a" and "b", this can be rewritten as

: T = a + b log_2 frac{D}{W}.

The above derivation is similar to one given in Card, Moran, and Newell (1983). For a critique of the deterministic iterative-corrections model, see Meyer et al. (1990).

See also

* Hick's law
* Point and click
* Accot-Zhai steering law
* Crossing-based interfaces

References

* Paul M. Fitts (1954). The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. "Journal of Experimental Psychology", volume 47, number 6, June 1954, pp. 381-391. (Reprinted in "Journal of Experimental Psychology: General", 121(3):262&ndash;269, 1992).
* Paul M. Fitts and James R. Peterson (1964). Information capacity of discrete motor responses. "Journal of Experimental Psychology", 67(2):103&ndash;112, February 1964.
* Stuart K. Card, William K. English, and Betty J. Burr (1978). Evaluation of mouse, rate-controlled isometric joystick, step keys, and text keys for text selection on a CRT. "Ergonomics", 21(8):601&ndash;613, 1978.
* I. Scott MacKenzie and William A. S. Buxton (1992). Extending Fitts' law to two-dimensional tasks. Proceedings of ACM CHI 1992 Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, pp. 219&ndash;226. http://doi.acm.org/10.1145/142750.142794
* A. Murata. Extending effective target width in Fitts' law to a two-dimensional pointing task. International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, 11(2):137&ndash;152, 1999. http://www.leaonline.com/doi/abs/10.1207/S153275901102_4
* Johnny Accot and Shumin Zhai (2003). Refining Fitts' law models for bivariate pointing. Proceedings of ACM CHI 2003 Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, pp. 193&ndash;200. http://doi.acm.org/10.1145/642611.642646
* Johnny Accot and Shumin Zhai (2002). More than dotting the i's &mdash; foundations for crossing-based interfaces. Proceedings of ACM CHI 2002 Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, pp. 73&ndash;80. http://doi.acm.org/10.1145/503376.503390
* Stuart K. Card, Thomas P. Moran, Allen Newell (1983). The Psychology of Human-Computer Interaction.
* I. Scott MacKenzie (1992). Fitts' law as a research and design tool in human-computer interaction. "Human-Computer Interaction", volume 7, 1992, pp. 91&ndash;139. http://www.yorku.ca/mack/hci1992.pdf
* Meyer, D. E., Smith, J. E. K., Kornblum, S., Abrams, R. A., & Wright, C. E. (1990). Speed-accuracy tradeoffs in aimed movements: Toward a theory of rapid voluntary action. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and performance XIII (pp. 173&ndash;226). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. http://www.umich.edu/~bcalab/Meyer_Bibliography.html
* A. T. Welford (1968). Fundamentals of Skill. Methuen, 1968.
* Shumin Zhai (2002). On the Validity of Throughput as a Characteristic of Computer Input, IBM Research Report RJ 10253, 2002, Almaden Research Center, San Jose, California. http://www.almaden.ibm.com/u/zhai/papers/ZhaiIBMReporRJ10253.pdf

External links

* [http://www.interaction-design.org/encyclopedia/fitts_law.html Peer-reviewed Encyclopedia entry on Fitts' Law] by Mehmet Göktürk
* [http://www.rodo.nl/fittsbits/ Fittsbits - A game investigating Fitts' Law] by Willem Vervuurt and Laura Cuijpers (this game, a Web-based experiment, is no longer available)
* [http://www.asktog.com/basics/firstPrinciples.html#fittsLaw Fitts' Law] at AskTog
* [http://www.asktog.com/columns/022DesignedToGiveFitts.html A Quiz Designed to Give You Fitts] at AskTog
* [http://ei.cs.vt.edu/~cs5724/g1/ Fitts' Law] at CS Dept. NSF-Supported Education Infrastructure Project
* [http://www.cs.umd.edu/class/fall2002/cmsc838s/tichi/fitts.html Fitts’ Law: Modeling Movement Time in HCI]
* [http://www.yorku.ca/mack/RN-Fitts_bib.htm Bibliography of Fitts’ Law Research] compiled by I. Scott MacKenzie
* [http://blogs.msdn.com/jensenh/archive/2006/08/22/711808.aspx Fitts' Law in Microsoft Office User Interface] by Jensen Harris
* [http://particletree.com/features/visualizing-fittss-law/ Visualizing Fitts's Law ] by Kevin Hale


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем решить контрольную работу

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Fitts Gesetz — Fitso dėsnis statusas T sritis Kūno kultūra ir sportas apibrėžtis Teiginys, nusakantis judesio trukmės, tikslumo ir taikinio nuotolio ryšį: greito ir tikslaus judesio trukmė priklauso nuo taikinio dydžio (norimo tikslumo laipsnio) ir atstumo iki… …   Sporto terminų žodynas

  • Paul Fitts — infobox scientist name = Paul Morris Fitts Jr. [1920 1930 Census] preferred name = Paul M. Fitts birth date = May 6, 1912 birth place = Martin, Tennessee death date = 1965 known for = Fitts s lawPaul M. Fitts (1912 – 1965) was a psychologist at… …   Wikipedia

  • Loi de Fitts — En psychologie expérimentale, en ergonomie et en interaction Homme machine, la loi de Fitts est un modèle du mouvement humain, prédisant le temps requis pour aller rapidement d une position de départ à une zone finale de destination, en fonction… …   Wikipédia en Français

  • Ley de Fitts — En ergonomía, la ley de Fitts es un modelo del movimiento humano, que predice el tiempo necesario para moverse rápidamente desde una posición inicial hasta una zona destino final como una función de la distancia hasta el objetivo y el tamaño de… …   Wikipedia Español

  • Steering law — The steering law is a predictive model of how quickly one may navigate, or steer , through a 2 dimensional tunnel. The tunnel can be thought of as a path or trajectory on a plane that has an associated thickness or width, where the width can vary …   Wikipedia

  • Buron Fitts — Buron Rogers Fitts (first name often incorrectly given as Burton ) (March 22, 1895 Belcherville, Texas March 1973 Three Rivers, California) was a California politician, who was lieutenant governor of the state from 1927 to 1928 and Los Angeles… …   Wikipedia

  • University of Pennsylvania Law School — Infobox University name = Penn Law established = 1790type = Private dean = Michael A.Fitts city = Philadelphia, Pennsylvania students = 848 faculty = campus = Urban website = http://law.upenn.edu The University of Pennsylvania Law School is the… …   Wikipedia

  • Hick's law — Hick s law, named after British psychologist W. E. Hick, or the Hick Hyman law, describes the time it takes for a person to make a decision as a function of the possible choices he or she has. Given n equally probable choices, the average… …   Wikipedia

  • Fitt's law — Fitso dėsnis statusas T sritis Kūno kultūra ir sportas apibrėžtis Teiginys, nusakantis judesio trukmės, tikslumo ir taikinio nuotolio ryšį: greito ir tikslaus judesio trukmė priklauso nuo taikinio dydžio (norimo tikslumo laipsnio) ir atstumo iki… …   Sporto terminų žodynas

  • Gesetz von Fitts — Fitso dėsnis statusas T sritis Kūno kultūra ir sportas apibrėžtis Teiginys, nusakantis judesio trukmės, tikslumo ir taikinio nuotolio ryšį: greito ir tikslaus judesio trukmė priklauso nuo taikinio dydžio (norimo tikslumo laipsnio) ir atstumo iki… …   Sporto terminų žodynas

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”