Conduction aphasia

Conduction aphasia
Conduction aphasia
Classification and external resources

Broca's area and Wernicke's area
MeSH D018886

Conduction aphasia, also called associative aphasia, is a relatively rare form of aphasia. An acquired language disorder, it is characterized by intact auditory comprehension, fluent (yet paraphasic) speech production, but poor speech repetition. Patients will display frequent errors during spontaneous speech, substituting or transposing sounds. They will also be aware of their errors, and will show significant difficulty correcting them.[1] Conduction aphasia usually is the result of damage to the left brain hemisphere, such as by stroke.

The classical explanation for conduction aphasia is that of a disconnection between the brain areas responsible for speech comprehension (Wernicke's area) and speech production (Broca's area), due specifically to damage to the arcuate fasciculus, a deep, white matter tract. Recent research has challenged this notion on the basis that patients with conduction aphasia more often have lesions in the supramarginal gyrus or deep parietal matter. However, conduction aphasia remains a controversial topic from the understanding of its neurologic foundations.

Contents

History

In the late 19th century, Paul Broca studied patients with expressive aphasia. These patients had lesions in the anterior perisylvian region (now known as Broca's area), and produced halting and labored speech, lacking in function words and grammar. Comprehension is generally preserved, although there can be deficits in interpretation of complex sentences. In an extreme example, one of his patients could only produce a single syllable, "Tan".

Meanwhile, Carl Wernicke described patients with receptive aphasia, who had damage to the left posterior superior temporal lobe, which he named "the area of word images". These patients could speak fluently, but their speech lacked meaning. They had a severe deficit in auditory comprehension. The two disorders (Broca's and Wernicke's aphasias) thus seemed complementary, and corresponded to two distinct anatomical locations.

Wernicke predicted the existence of conduction aphasia in his landmark 1874 monograph, Der Aphasische Symptomenkompleks: Eine Psychologische Studie auf Anatomischer Basis.[1][2][3] He was the first to distinguish the various aphasias in an anatomical framework, and proposed that a disconnection between the two speech systems (motor and sensory) would lead to a unique condition, distinct from both Broca's and Wernicke's aphasias, which he termed Leitungsaphasie. He did not explicitly predict the repetition deficit, but did note that, unlike those with Wernicke's aphasia, conduction aphasics would be able to comprehend speech properly, and intriguingly, would be able to hear and understand their own speech errors, leading to frustration and self-correction.[2][4]

Wernicke was influenced by Theodor Meynert, his mentor, who postulated that aphasias were due to perisylvian lesions. Meynert also distinguished between the posterior and anterior language systems, leading Wernicke to localize the two regions.[3] Wernicke's research into the fiber pathways connecting the posterior and anterior regions lead him to theorize that damage to the fibers under the insula would lead to conduction aphasia. Ludwig Lichtheim expanded on Wernicke's work, although he labeled the disorder commissural aphasia, to distinguish between aphasias tied to processing centers.[5]

Sigmund Freud would argue in 1891 that the old framework was inaccurate; the entire perisylvian area, from the posterior to the anterior regions, were equivalent in facilitating speech function. In 1948 Kurt Goldstein postulated that spoken language was a central phenomenon, as opposed to a differentiated and disparate set of functionally distinct modules. To Freud and Goldstein, conduction aphasia was thus the result of a central, core language breakdown; Goldstein labeled the disorder central aphasia.[3]

Later work and examination of brain structures, however, implicated the arcuate fasciculus, a white matter bundle connecting the posterior temporoparietal junction with the frontal cortex. Norman Geschwind proposed that damage to this bundle caused conduction aphasia; the characteristic deficits in auditory repetition were due to failed transmission of information between the two language centers.[3] Studies showed that conduction aphasics had an intact 'inner voice', which descredited the central deficit model of Freud and Goldstein.[6] The Wernicke-Lichtheim-Geschwind disconnection hypothesis thus became the prevailing explanation for conduction aphasia. However, recent reviews and research have cast doubt on the singular role of the arcuate fasciculus and the model of spoken language in general[citation needed].

Presentation

Conduction aphasics will show relatively well-preserved auditory comprehension, which may even be completely functional. Spontaneous speech production will be fluent and generally grammatically and syntactically correct. Intonation and articulation will also be preserved. Speech will often contain paraphasic errors: phonemes and syllables will be dropped or transposed (e.g., "snowball" → "snowall", "television" → "vellitision", "ninety-five percent" → "ninety-twenty percent"). The hallmark deficit of this disorder, however, is in repetition. Patients will show a marked inability to repeat words or sentences when prompted by an examiner.[7][8] When prompted to repeat words, patients will be unable to do so, and produce many paraphasic errors. For example, when prompted with "bagger", a patient may respond with, "gabber".[9] Oral reading can also be poor.

Patients recognize their paraphasias and errors, and will attempt to correct them; this repetitive effort to approximate the appropriate phrase is known as conduite d’approche.[8] For example, when prompted to repeat "Rosenkranz", a patient may respond with, "rosenbrau... rosenbrauch... rosengrau... bro... grosenbrau... grossenlau, rosenkranz,... kranz... rosenkranz".[9]

Conduction aphasia is a relatively mild language impairment, and most patients return to day-to-day life.[9][10] Symptoms of conduction aphasia, as with other aphasias, can be transient, lasting only several hours or a few days. As aphasias and other language disorders are frequently due to stroke, their symptoms can change and evolve over time, or simply disappear. This is due to healing in the brain after inflammation or hemorrhage, which leads to decreased local impairment. Furthermore, plastic changes in the brain may lead to the recruitment of new pathways to restore lost function. However, chronic conduction aphasia is possible, without transformation to other aphasias.[9] These patients show prolonged, profound deficits in repetition, frequent phonemic paraphasias, and conduite d'approche during spontaneous speech.

Diagnosis

Conduction aphasics are capable of normal conversation, so freely conversing is the first opportunity for noticing signs of the disorder.[8] Patients will speak normally, but include occasional paraphasias. When asked to repeat something, the patient will be unable to do so without significant difficulty, repeatedly attempting to self-correct (conduite d'approche). When asked a question, however, patients can answer spontaneously and fluently.

Several standardized test batteries exist for diagnosing and classifying aphasias. These tests are capable of identifying conduction aphasia with relative accuracy.[8] The Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE) and the Western Aphasia Battery (WAB) are two commonly used test batteries for diagnosing conduction aphasia. These examinations involve a set of tests, which include asking patients to name pictures, read printed words, count aloud, and repeat words and non-words (such as shwazel).

Treatment

Treatment for aphasias is generally individualized, focusing on specific language and communication improvements, and regular exercise with communication tasks. Regular therapy for conduction aphasics has been shown to result in steady improvement on the Western Aphasia Battery.[11] However, conduction aphasia is a mild aphasia, and conduction aphasics score highly on the WAB at baseline.

Causes

Conduction aphasia is caused by damage in the language-dominant hemisphere (the left hemisphere in most individuals). Lesions in the language centers (and connections between) have been identified as potential inducers for conduction aphasia (and other language disorders, in general). Specifically, patterns of damage in conduction aphasia patients have been observed to cluster in the posterior and inferior temporal lobe, and in the parieto-temporal junction.[7][9]

The brain damage causing conduction aphasia is often from a stroke, which can produce both localized and widespread damage. Traumatic brain injury and tumors can also lead to localized lesioning, with potential to cause conduction aphasia.

Pathophysiology

Traditionally, it has been believed that conduction aphasia was the result of a lesion in the arcuate fasciculus, a deep, white matter bundle connecting the posterior temporoparietal junction with the frontal cortex. It was thought that this bundle transmitted information between Wernicke's area (responsible for language comprehension) and Broca's area (responsible for language production). Wernicke, and later Lichtheim and others, theorized that a disconnect between these two regions caused patients to fail to monitor speech and limited their ability to transfer information between comprehension and production functions, thus leading to paraphasic errors and a deficit in repetition of auditory input. This hypothesis fits well with the Wernicke-Geschwind model of language, which compartmentalizes and localizes speech comprehension and production.

Although the disconnection hypothesis explains many of the conditions associated with conduction aphasia, clinical evidence is lacking, and the Wernicke-Geschwind model has since become obsolete. There have been no known autopsy cases in which conduction aphasia was shown to be the result of a focused arcuate fasciculus lesion.[12] Surveys of conduction aphasics with anatomical confirmation show that in nearly all patients, there was damage to portions of the cortex as well. Furthermore, there are reports of patients with severe disruption of the arcuate fasciculus who show no symptoms of conduction aphasia (although it is plausible that the contralateral hemisphere facilitated repetition in these cases).[13]

Recent research has pointed to a different explanation for conduction aphasia, similar to Wernicke's, which is based on newer models suggesting language is facilitated by "cortically based, anatomically distributed, modular networks."[3] Anderson et al. describe an experiment in which electrical stimulation of the left posterior superior temporal cortex in a human subject induced symptoms consistent with conduction aphasia, indicating that a deep brain disconnection is not necessary.[3] While this study does not completely discredit the disconnection hypothesis, but does point to a system in which transmission of spoken language information involves more than just the arcuate fasciculus. Regardless of the role that the arcuate fasciculus plays in the disorder, the cortical component cannot be denied.

Current theory

It has been shown that Broca's and Wernicke's aphasias are not due to damage specific to the areas described as Broca's and Wernicke's areas. The symptoms associated with these two disorders are much more diverse and disparate than can be accounted for by a single lesion in a single processing module. Broca's aphasics, for example, have difficulty with speech and with comprehension of complex grammar, two distinct functions. Furthermore, it is likely that inaccurate localization of lesions (the two disorders were first studied in the 19th century) has led to incorrect identification of causal damage.[1] Rather, it is likely that language comprehension and production are mediated by a more distributed system, involving multiple pathways and multiple cortical modules. Their relative positions may have led to an oversimplification of the system into two distinct and functionally autonomous centers, with a single connecting pathway.

One current theory holds that there are two distinct routes for language processing: auditory and visual. Each route has two pathways; the auditory route is separated into the auditory-phonological conversion pathway and the auditory word analysis pathway. Conduction aphasia is postulated to be a breakdown in the auditory-phonological pathway. Patients retain the auditory word analysis pathway, and thus have preserved access to lexical/semantic information. In other words, they are perfectly capable of understanding the meaning of auditory input. However, patients lack the auditory-phonological pathway, and are unable to convert auditory input into the appropriate phonemes, thus hindering repetition and rhyme judgment, especially of nonsense words (which have no meaning that they can access). Furthermore, semantic intrusions (paraphasias) are frequent, due to preservation of meaning but not phonology ("ninety-five" → "ninety-twenty").

The model predicts a similar, complementary syndrome to conduction aphasia, where patients could not comprehend auditory input, yet could repeat it, through preservation of the auditory word analysis pathway, but damage to the auditory-phonological conversion pathway.[1] This is known as transcortical sensory aphasia, in which patients are capable of repeating what is heard (and correcting grammatical errors), yet have difficulty understanding what was said. These patients have lesions in the supramarginal and angular gyri. A related disorder is echolalia, a symptom of autism, schizophrenia, and Tourette syndrome. Patients will repeat what is heard, sometimes involuntarily, with little care for meaning.

The anatomical framework for this theory is currently unclear. It is difficult to localize the disparate language functions, despite recent advances in imaging techniques (such as fMRI and PET. The more posterior regions (such as around the temporoparietal junction) seem to correlate to functions associated with deficits shown by Wernicke's aphasics, while anterior regions (the perisylvian gyrus) appear to correlate with those of Broca's aphasics. It is likely that language is distributed in such a way that there is no distinct region for any of the multiple, interconnected functions.

See also

References

  1. ^ a b c d Gazzaniga, Michael S.; Ivry, Richard B.; George R. Mangun; (2002). Cognitive neuroscience: the biology of the mind. New York: W. W. Norton. pp. 389. ISBN 0-393-97777-3. 
  2. ^ a b Kohn, Susan E. (1992). Conduction aphasia. Hillsdale, N.J: L. Erlbaum. pp. 25–26. ISBN 0-8058-0681-4. 
  3. ^ a b c d e f Anderson JM, Gilmore R, Roper S, et al. (October 1999). "Conduction aphasia and the arcuate fasciculus: A reexamination of the Wernicke-Geschwind model". Brain Lang 70 (1): 1–12. doi:10.1006/brln.1999.2135. PMID 10534369. 
  4. ^ Kohler K, Bartels C, Herrman M, et al. (October 1998). "Conduction aphasia--11 classic cases". Aphasiology 12 (10): 865–884. doi:10.1080/02687039808249456. 
  5. ^ Kohn, Susan E. (1992). Conduction aphasia. Hillsdale, N.J: L. Erlbaum. pp. 28–29. ISBN 0-8058-0681-4. 
  6. ^ Feinberg TE, Gonzalez Rothi LJ, Heilman KM (June 1986). "'Inner speech' in conduction aphasia". Arch. Neurol. 43 (6): 591–3. PMID 3718287. 
  7. ^ a b Damasio H, Damasio AR (June 1980). "The anatomical basis of conduction aphasia". Brain 103 (2): 337–50. doi:10.1093/brain/103.2.337. PMID 7397481. http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=7397481. 
  8. ^ a b c d Kohn, Susan E. (1992). Conduction aphasia. Hillsdale, N.J: L. Erlbaum. pp. 40–42. ISBN 0-8058-0681-4. 
  9. ^ a b c d e Bartha L, Benke T (April 2003). "Acute conduction aphasia: an analysis of 20 cases". Brain Lang 85 (1): 93–108. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(02)00502-3. PMID 12681350. http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0093934X02005023. 
  10. ^ Benson DF, Sheremata WA, Bouchard R, Segarra JM, Price D, Geschwind N (May 1973). "Conduction aphasia. A clinicopathological study". Arch. Neurol. 28 (5): 339–46. PMID 4696016. 
  11. ^ Bakheit AM, Shaw S, Carrington S, Griffiths S (October 2007). "The rate and extent of improvement with therapy from the different types of aphasia in the first year after stroke". Clin Rehabil 21 (10): 941–9. doi:10.1177/0269215507078452. PMID 17981853. 
  12. ^ Tanabe H, Sawada T, Inoue N, Ogawa M, Kuriyama Y, Shiraishi J (December 1987). "Conduction aphasia and arcuate fasciculus". Acta Neurol. Scand. 76 (6): 422–7. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0404.1987.tb03597.x. PMID 3434200. 
  13. ^ Shuren JE, Schefft BK, Yeh HS, Privitera MD, Cahill WT, Houston W (September 1995). "Repetition and the arcuate fasciculus". J. Neurol. 242 (9): 596–8. doi:10.1007/BF00868813. PMID 8551322. 

External links


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Нужна курсовая?

Look at other dictionaries:

  • conduction aphasia — noun aphasia in which the lesion is assumed to be in the association tracts connecting the various language centers in the brain; patient s have difficulty repeating a sentence just heard • Syn: ↑associative aphasia • Hypernyms: ↑aphasia …   Useful english dictionary

  • conduction aphasia — a type of aphasia characterized by normal comprehension but inability to repeat words correctly; said to be caused by lesions in the pathways connecting the Broca motor speech and Wernicke areas. Called also associative a. and commissural a …   Medical dictionary

  • Aphasia — For other uses, see Aphasia (disambiguation). Aphasia Classification and external resources ICD 10 F80.0 F80.2, R …   Wikipedia

  • Aphasia — Literally aphasia means no speech. Aphasia can apply to a defect in expression or comprehension. * * * Impaired or absent comprehension or production of, or communication by, speech, writing, or signs, due to an acquired lesion of the dominant… …   Medical dictionary

  • Expressive aphasia — Classification and external resources Broca s area and Wernicke s area ICD 10 F80 …   Wikipedia

  • Nominal aphasia — Anomic aphasia Classification and external resources Diffusion tensor imaging image of the brain showing the right and left arcuate fasciculus (Raf Laf). Also shown are the right and left superior longitudinal fasciculus (Rslf Lslf) …   Wikipedia

  • Receptive aphasia — DiseaseDisorder infobox Name = Receptive aphasia Caption = Broca s area and Wernicke s area ICD10 = ICD10|F|80|2|f|80 ICD9 = ICD9|315.32 ICDO = OMIM = DiseasesDB = MedlinePlus = eMedicineSubj = eMedicineTopic = MeshID = D001041 Receptive aphasia …   Wikipedia

  • associative aphasia — noun aphasia in which the lesion is assumed to be in the association tracts connecting the various language centers in the brain; patient s have difficulty repeating a sentence just heard • Syn: ↑conduction aphasia • Hypernyms: ↑aphasia …   Useful english dictionary

  • transcortical aphasia — a type of conduction aphasia believed to be caused by a lesion of a pathway between the speech center and other cortical centers, but often reflecting large lesions in brain areas other than the perisylvian region of the hemisphere dominant for… …   Medical dictionary

  • associative aphasia — conduction a …   Medical dictionary

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”