Visual modularity

Visual modularity

In cognitive neuroscience, visual modularity is an organizational concept concerning how vision works. The way in which the primate visual system operates is currently under intense scientific scrutiny. One dominant thesis is that different properties of the visual world (colour, motion, form and so forth) require different computational solutions which are implemented in anatomically/functionally distinct regions that operate independently – that is, in a modular fashion (Calabretta & Parisi, 2005).

Motion processing

Akinetopsia is an intriguing condition brought about by damage to the extra-striate area hMT+ that renders humans and monkeys unable to perceive motion (Zihl et al., 1983, 1991) and indicates that there might be a “motion centre” in the brain. Of course, such data can only indicate that this area is at least necessary to motion perception, not that it is sufficient; however, other evidence has shown the importance of this area to primate motion perception. Specifically, physiological, neuroimaging, perceptual, electrical- and transcranial magnetic stimulation evidence (Table 1) all come together on the area V5/hMT+. Converging evidence of this type is supportive of a module for motion processing. However, this view is likely to be incomplete: other areas are involved with motion perception, including V1 (Orban et al., 1986; Movshon & Newsome, 1996, Born and Bradley, 2005), V2 and V3a (see Grill-Spector and Malach, 2004) and areas surrounding V5/hMT+ (Table 2). A recent fMRI study put the number of motion areas at twenty-one (Stiers et al., 2006:83). Clearly a stream of diverse anatomical areas subserves motion perception. However, the extent to which this is ‘pure’ is in question: with Akinetopsia come severe difficulties in obtaining structure from motion (Rizzo, Nawrot, Zihl, 1995). V5/hMT+ has since been implicated in this function (Grunewald, Bradley & Andersen, 2002) as well as determining depth (DeAngelis, Cumming and Newsome, 1998). Thus the current evidence suggests that motion processing occurs in a modular stream, although with a role in form and depth perception at higher levels.

Table 1 | Evidence for a “motion centre” in the primate brain

Form processing

Another clinical case that would a priori suggest a module for modularity in visual processing is visual agnosia. The well studied patient DF is unable to recognize or discriminate objects (Mishkin, Ungerleider and Macko, 1983) owing to damage in areas of the lateral occipital cortex (James et al., 2003) although she can see scenes without problem – she can literally see the forest but not the trees (Steeves et al. 2006). Neuroimaging of intact individuals reveals strong occipito-temporal activation during object presentation and greater activation still for object recognition (see Grill-Spector, 2003). Of course, such activation could be due to other processes, such as visual attention. However, other evidence that shows a tight coupling of perceptual and physiological changes (Sheinberg and Logothetis, 2001) suggests activation in this area does underpin object recognition. Within these regions are more specialized areas for face or fine grained analysis (Gauthier, Skudlarski, Gore and Anderson, 2000), place perception (Epstein & Kanwisher, 1998) and human body perception (Downing, Jiang, Shuman and Kanwisher, 2001). Perhaps some of the strongest evidence for the modular nature of these processing systems is the double dissociation between object- and face (prosop-) agnosia (e.g. Moscowitch, Winocur and Behrmann, 1997). However, as with color and motion, early areas (see Pasupathy, 2006 for a comprehensive review) are implicated too lending support to the idea of a multistage stream terminating in the inferotemporal cortex rather than an isolated module.

Functional modularity

One of the first uses of the term "module" or "modularity" occurs in the influential book "Modularity of Mind" by the Philosopher Jerry Fodor (1983). A detailed application of this idea to the case of vision was published by Pylyshyn (1999), who argued that there is a significant part of vision that is not responsive to beliefs and is "cognitively impenetrable."

Much of the confusion concerning modularity exists in neuroscience because there is evidence for specific areas (e.g. V4 or V5/hMT+) and the concomitant behavioral deficits following brain insult (thus taken as evidence for modularity). In addition, evidence shows other areas are involved and that these areas subserve processing of multiple properties (e.g. V1: see Leventhal et al, 1995) (thus taken as evidence against modularity). That these streams have the same implementation in early visual areas, like V1 is not inconsistent with a modular viewpoint: to adopt the canonical analogy in cognition, it is possible for different software to run on the same hardware. A consideration of psychophysics and neuropsychological data would suggest support for this. For example, psychophysics has shown that percepts for different properties are realized asynchronously (Moutoussis & Zeki 1997, Viviani & Aymoz, 2001). In addition, although achromats experience other cognitive defects (Gegenfurtner, 2003) they do not have motion or form deficits when their lesion is restricted to V4 (Zeki, 2005). Relatedly, Zihl and colleagues’ (1983) Akinetopsia patient shows no deficit to color or object perception (although deriving depth and structure from motion is problematic, see above) and object agnostics do not have damaged motion or color perception, making the three disorders triply dissociable. Taken together this evidence suggests that even though distinct properties may employ the same early visual areas they are functionally independent. Furthermore, that the intensity of subjective perceptual experience (e.g. color) correlates with activity in these specific areas (e.g. V4) (Bartels and Zeki, 2005), the recent evidence that synaesthetes show V4 activation during the perceptual experience of color, as well as the fact that damage to these areas results in concomitant behavioral deficits (the processing may be occurring but perceivers do not have access to the information) are all evidence for visual modularity.

References

Bartels, A & Zeki, S. (2000). The architecture of the human color centre: new results and a review. European Journal of Neuroscience, 12(1), 172-193.

Bartels, A. & Zeki, S. (2005). Brain dynamics during natural viewing conditions - a new guide for mapping connectivity "in vivo". NeuroImage, 24(2), 339-349.

Beauchamp, M.S., Haxby, J.V., Rosen, A.C. & DeYoe, E.A.A. (2000). MRI functional case study of acquired cerebral dyschromatopsia. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1170–1179.

Beauchamp, M.S., Lee, K.E., Haxby, J.V., & Martin, A. (2003) FMRI responses to video and point-light displays of moving humans and manipulable objects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 991-1001.

Beckers, G. & Zeki, S. 1995 The consequences of inactivating areas V1 and V5 on visual motion perception. Brain, 118, 49–60.

Born, R. T. & Bradley, D. C. (2005) Structure and function of visual area MT. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28,157-89.

Bouvier, S.E. & Engel, S.A. (2006). Behavioral Deficits and Cortical Damage Loci in Cerebral Achromatopsia. Cerebral Cortex, 16(2),183-191.

Büchel, C., Josephs, O., Rees, G. Turner, R., Frith, C. D., & Friston, K. J. (1998). The functional anatomy of attention to visual motion. A functional MRI study. Brain, 121(7), 1281-94.

Calabretta, R. & Parisi, D. (2005). Evolutionary Connectionism and Mind/Brain Modularity. In W. Callabaut & D. Rasskin-Gutman , Modularity. Understanding the development and evolution of complex natural systems (pp. 309-330). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Chelazzi, L., Miller, E. K., Duncan, J., & Desimone, R. (2001). Responses of neurons in macaque area V4 during memory-guided visual search. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 761-772.

Conway, B.R., Tsao, D.Y. (2006). Color architecture in alert macaque cortex revealed by fMRI. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 1604–1613.

Culham, J.C., Brandt, S.A., Cavanagh, P., Kanwisher, N.G., Dale, A.M., & Tootell, R.B.H. (1998) Cortical fMRI activation produced by attentive tracking of moving targets. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80, 2657–2670.

DeAngelis, G.C., Cumming, B.G., & Newsome, W.T. (1998) A new role for cortical area MT: The perception of stereoscopic depth. Nature, 394, 677-680.

DeYoe, E.A. & Van Essen, D.C. (1985). Segregation of efferent connections and receptive field properties in visual area V2 of the macaque. Nature, 317, 58−61.

Downing, P., Jiang, Y., Shuman, M., & Kanwisher, N. (2001). A Cortical Area Selective for Visual Processing of the Human Body. Science, 293, 2470-2473.

Epstein, N., & Kanwisher, N. (1998). A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature, 392(6676), 598-601.

Felleman, D.J. & Kaas, J.H. (1984). Receptive field properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area (MT) of owl monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology, 52, 488-513.

Fodor, J. A. (1983). The Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, a Bradford Book.

Gauthier, I., Skudlarski, P., Gore, J. C., & Anderson, A. W. (2000). Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition. Nature Neuroscience, 3(2), 191-197.

Gegenfurtner, K.R. (2003) Cortical mechanisms of color vision. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 563-572.

Gowers, W. (1888) A manual of diseases of the brain. London: J. & A. Churchill.

Grill-Spector, K. & Malach, R. (2004). The human visual cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 7, 649-677.

Grill-Spector, K. (2003). The neural basis of object perception. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13(3), 399-399.

Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., & Blake, R. (2000) Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 711-20.

Grunewald, A, Bradley, D.C., & Andersen, R.A. (2002). Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 6195-6207.

Grüsser, O.J., & Landis, T. (1991) The splitting of ‘‘I’’ and ‘‘me’’: Heautoscopy and related phenomena. In: Visual agnosias and other disturbances of visual perception and cognition. Amsterdam: MacMillan: 297–303.

Hotson, J., Braun, D., Herzberg, W., & Boman, D. (1994). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of extrastriate cortex degrades human motion direction discrimination. Vision Research, 34(16), 2115-23.

Kusunoki, M., Moutoussis, K., Zeki, S. (2006) Effect of background colors on the tuning of color-selective cells in monkey area v4. Journal of Neurophysiology, 95(5), 3047-3059.

Leventhal, A.G., Thompson, K.G., Liu, D., Zhou, Y., & Ault, S.J. (1995). Concomitant sensitivity to orientation, direction, and color of cells in layers 2, 3, and 4 of monkey striate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 1808–1818.

Livingstone, M.S. & Hubel, D.H. (1984). Anatomy and physiology of a color system in the primate visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 309–356.

Maunsell, J.H.R. & Van Essen, D.C. (1983). Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed and orientation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 49, 1127-1147.

Meadows, J. C. (1974) Disturbed perception of colours associated with localized cerebral lesions. Brain, 97, 615–632.

Mishkin, M., Ungerleider, L., & Macko, K. (1983), Object vision and spatial vision: Two cortical pathways. Trends in Neuroscience, 6, 414-417.

Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., Behrmann, M. (1997). What is special about face recognition? Nineteen experiments on a person with visual object agnosia and dyslexia but normal face recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 555–604.

Moutoussis, K., & Zeki, S. (1997). A direct demonstration of perceptual asynchrony in vision. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, 264(1380), 393–399.

Movshon, J.A., & Newsome, W.T. 1996. Visual response properties of striate cortical neurons projecting to area MT in macaque monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience, 16(23), 7733-41.

Orban, G.A., Kennedy, H., & Bullier, J. (1986). Velocity sensitivity and direction selectivity of neurons in areas V1 and V2 of the monkey: influence of eccentricity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 56(2), 462-80.

Pasupathy, A. (2006). Neural basis of shape representation in the primate brain. In: Visual Perception (Part 1): Progress in Brain Research, Martinez-Conde, Macknik, Martinez, Alonso & Tse, Eds.

Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1999). Is vision continuous with cognition? The case for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(3), 341-423.

Rich, A.N., Williams, M.A., Puce, A., Syngeniotis, A., Howard, M.A., McGlone, F., Mattingley, J.B. (2006). Neural correlates of imagined and synaesthetic colours. Neuropsychologia, 44(14), 2918-25.

Rizzo, M., Nawrot, M., & Zihl, J. (1995) Motion and shape perception in cerebral akinetopsia. Brain, 118,1105-27.

Sacks, O., & Wasserman, R. L. (1987). The painter who became color blind. New York Review of Books, 34, 25-33.

Salzman, C.D., Murasugi, C.M., Britten, K.H., & Newsome, W.T. (1992) Microstimulation in visual area MT: effects on direction discrimination performance. Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 2331-2356.

Sheinberg, D.L., & Logothetis, N. K. (2001). Noticing Familiar Objects in Real World Scenes: The Role of Temporal Cortical Neurons in Natural Vision. Journal of Neuroscience, 21(4), 1340 – 1350.

Sperling, J.M., Prvulovic, D., Linden, D.E., Singer, W., & Stirn, A. (2006). Neuronal correlates of colour-graphemic synaesthesia: an fMRI study. Cortex, 42(2), 295-303

Steeves, J.K.E., Culham, J.C., DuChaine, B.C., C. Cavina Pratesi, Valyear, K., Schindler, I., Humphrey, G.K., Milner, A.D. & Goodale, M.A. (2006). The fusiform face area is not sufficient for face recognition: evidence from a patient with dense prosopagnosia and no occipital face area. Neuropsychologia, 44(4), 596-609.

Stephen V. David, S.V., Hayden, B.Y., & Gallant, J.L (2006) Spectral Receptive Field Properties Explain Shape Selectivity in Area V4. Journal of Neurophysiology, 96, 3492-3505.

Stiers, P., Peeters, R., Lagae, L., Van Hecke, P., & Sunaertc, S. (2006) Mapping multiple visual areas in the human brain with a short fMRI sequence. NeuroImage, 29, 74 – 89.

Tanaka, K. & Saito, H.A. (1989) Analysis of motion of the visual field by direction, expansion/contraction, and rotation cells clustered in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology, 62, 626–641.

Tootell, R., Nelissen, K., Vanduffel, W., & Orban, G. (2004). Search for color 'center(s)' in macaque visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 353-363

Van Essen D.C., Maunsell, J.H.R., & Bixby, J.L. (1981). The middle temporal visual area in the macaque: myeloarchitecture, connections, functional properties and topographic organization. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 199, 293–326.

Viviani, P., & Aymoz, C. (2001). Color, form and movement are not perceived simultaneously. Vision Research, 41(22), 2909–2918.

Wachtler, T., Sejnowski, T. J., & Albright, T. D. (2003) Representation of color stimuli in awake macaque primary visual cortex. Neuron, 37, 681–691.

Wade, A.R. & Wandell B.A. (2002). Chromatic Light Adaptation Measured using Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 22(18):8148-8157.

Walsh, V., & Cowey, A. (1998). Magnetic stimulation studies of visual cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 103–110.

Zeki S. (2005). The Ferrier Lecture 1995 behind the seen: The functional specialization of the brain in space and time. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci., 360, 1145-83.

Zeki, S. (1990) Parallelism and functional specialization in human visual cortex. Quant. Biol. 55, 651–661.

Zeki, S. M. (1974) Functional organization of a visual area in the posterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesus monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology, 236, 549–573.

Zeki, S.M. & Marini, L (1998). Three cortical stages of color processing in the human brain. Brain, 121, 1669–1685.

Zihl, J., von Cramon D., Mai, N., & Schmid, C. (1991). Disturbance of movement vision after bilateral posterior brain damage. Further evidence and follow up observations. Brain, 114, 2235–52.

Zihl, J., von Cramon, D., & Mai, N. (1983). Selective disturbances of movement vision after bilateral brain damage. Brain, 106, 313–40.

ee also

*Modularity
*Society of Mind which proposes the mind is made up of s


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем решить контрольную работу

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Modularity of mind — The concept of modularity is also used in other fields. See modularity. Modularity of mind is the notion that a mind may, at least in part, be composed of separate innate structures which have established evolutionarily developed functional… …   Wikipedia

  • Modularity — Module redirects here. For other uses, see Module (disambiguation). Modularity is a general systems concept, typically defined as a continuum describing the degree to which a system’s components may be separated and recombined.[1] It refers to… …   Wikipedia

  • Visual system — The visual system includes the eyes, the connecting pathways through to the visual cortex and other parts of the brain. The illustration shows the mammalian system. The visual system is the part of the central nervous system which enables… …   Wikipedia

  • Functional specialization (brain) — This article is about specialization of function among regions of the brain generally. For the particular topic of left right brain specialization, see Lateralization of brain function. Functional specialization suggests that different areas in… …   Wikipedia

  • Cognitive module — A cognitive module is, in theories of the modularity of mind and the closely related society of mind theory, a specialised tool or sub unit that can be used by other parts to resolve cognitive tasks. The question of their existence and nature is… …   Wikipedia

  • Modular art — is art created by joining together standardized units (modules) to form larger, more complex compositions. In some works the units can be subsequently moved, removed and added to – that is, modulated – to create a new work of art, different from… …   Wikipedia

  • Multimodal integration — Multimodal integration, also known as multisensory integration, is the study of how information from the different sensory modalities, such as sight, sound, touch, smell, self motion and taste, may be integrated by the nervous system. A coherent… …   Wikipedia

  • List of programming languages by category — Programming language lists Alphabetical Categorical Chronological Generational This is a list of programming languages grouped by category. Some languages are listed in multiple categories. Contents …   Wikipedia

  • Dynamic-link library — This article is about the OS/2 and Windows implementation. For dynamic linking of libraries in general, see Dynamic linker. Dynamic link library Filename extension .dll Internet media type application/x msd …   Wikipedia

  • Language module — refers to a hypothesized structure in the human brain (anatomical module) or cognitive system (functional module) that some psycholinguists (e.g., Steven Pinker) claim contains innate capacities for language. According to Jerry Fodor the sine qua …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”