Kohlberg's stages of moral development

Kohlberg's stages of moral development

Kohlberg's stages of moral development are planes of moral adequacy conceived by Lawrence Kohlberg to explain the development of moral reasoning. Created while studying psychology at the University of Chicago, the theory was inspired by the work of Jean Piaget and a fascination with children's reactions to moral dilemmas.cite book | last=Crain | first=William C. | title=Theories of Development | edition=2Rev Ed | url=http://faculty.plts.edu/gpence/html/kohlberg.htm | publisher=Prentice-Hall | year=1985 |id=ISBN 0-13-913617-7 ] He wrote his doctoral dissertation at the university in 1958,cite journal | last=Kohlberg | first=Lawrence | title=The Development of Modes of Thinking and Choices in Years 10 to 16 | journal=Ph. D. dissertation, University of Chicago | year=1958 ] outlining what are now known as his stages of moral development.

His theory holds that moral reasoning, which is the basis for ethical behavior, has six identifiable developmental constructive stages - each more adequate at responding to moral dilemmas than the last. In studying these, Kohlberg followed the development of moral judgment far beyond the ages originally studied earlier by Piaget,cite book | last=Piaget | first=Jean | title=The Moral Judgment of the Child | publisher=Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner and Co. | location=London | year=1932 |id=ISBN 0-02-925240-7 ] who also claimed that logic and morality develop through constructive stages.cite journal | last=Kohlberg | first=Lawrence | title=The Claim to Moral Adequacy of a Highest Stage of Moral Judgment | journal=Journal of Philosophy | volume=70 | year=1973 | pages=630–646 | doi=10.2307/2025030 ] Expanding considerably upon this groundwork, it was determined that the process of moral development was principally concerned with justice and that its development continued throughout the lifespan, even spawning dialogue on philosophical implications of such research.cite book |last=Kohlberg |first=Lawrence |title= Essays on Moral Development, Vol. I: The Philosophy of Moral Development |publisher=Harper & Row |Location=San Francisco, CA |year=1981 |id=ISBN 0-06-064760-4 ] cite book | last=Kohlberg | first=Lawrence | authorlink=Lawrence Kohlberg | coauthors=Charles Levine, Alexandra Hewer | title=Moral stages : a current formulation and a response to critics | publisher=Karger | location=Basel, NY | year=1983 |id=ISBN 3-8055-3716-6]

Kohlberg used stories about moral dilemmas in his studies, and was interested in how people would justify their actions if they were put in a similar moral crux. He would then categorize and classify evoked responses into one of six distinct stages. These six stages are grouped into three levels: pre-conventional, conventional and post-conventional.cite book |last=Kohlberg |first=Lawrence |title= From "Is" to "Ought": How to Commit the Naturalistic Fallacy and Get Away with It in the Study of Moral Development |publisher=Academic Press |Location=New York |year=1971 ] cite book |last=Kohlberg |first=Lawrence | coauthors=T. Lickona, ed. |title=Moral Development and Behavior: Theory, Research and Social Issues |publisher=Rinehart and Winston |Location=Holt, NY |year=1976 |chapter= Moral stages and moralization: The cognitive-developmental approach ] cite book |last=Colby |first=Anne | coauthors=Kohlberg, L. |title=The Measurement of Moral Judgment Vol. 2: Standard Issue Scoring Manual |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=1987 | id=ISBN 0-521-24447-1]

tages

Kohlberg's six stages were grouped into three levels: pre-conventional, conventional, and post-conventional. Following Piaget's constructivist requirements for a stage model (see his theory of cognitive development), it is extremely rare to regress backward in stages - to lose functionality of higher stage abilities.cite journal |last=Walker |first=Lawrence, J. |year=1989 |month=February |title=A longitudinal study of moral reasoning |journal=Child Development |volume=60 |issue=1 |pages=157–166 |doi=10.2307/1131081 ] cite book | author=Anne Colby | coauthors=Gibbs, J. Lieberman, M., and Kohlberg, L. | title=A Longitudinal Study of Moral Judgment: A Monograph for the Society of Research in Child Development| publisher=The University of Chicago Press | location: Chicago, IL| year=1983| id=ISBN 99932-7-870-X] Even so, no one functions at their highest stage at all times.Fact|date=February 2007 It is also not possible to 'jump' forward stages; each stage provides a new yet necessary perspective, and is more comprehensive, differentiated, and integrated than its predecessors.

:"Level 1 (Pre-Conventional)":::1. Obedience and punishment orientation::::: ("How can I avoid punishment?"):::2. Self-interest orientation::::: ("What's in it for me?")

:"Level 2 (Conventional)"

:::3. Interpersonal accord and conformity::::: ("Social norms")::::: ("The good boy/good girl attitude"):::4. Authority and social-order maintaining orientation::::: ("Law and order morality")

:"Level 3 (Post-Conventional)"

:::5. Social contract orientation:::6. Universal ethical principles::::: ("Principled conscience")

Pre-Conventional

The pre-conventional level of moral reasoning is especially common in children, although adults can also exhibit this level of reasoning. Reasoners in the pre-conventional level judge the morality of an action by its direct consequences. The pre-conventional level consists of the first and second stages of moral development, and are purely concerned with the self in an egocentric manner.

In "Stage one" (obedience and punishment driven), individuals focus on the direct consequences that their actions will have for themselves. For example, an action is perceived as morally wrong if the person who commits it gets punished. The worse the punishment for the act is, the more 'bad' the act is perceived to be.cite book | last=Shaffer | first=David R. | title=Social and Personality Development | edition=5th Ed | publisher=Wadsworth Publishing | year=2004 |id=ISBN 0-534-60700-4 ] This can give rise to an inference that even innocent victims are guilty in proportion to their suffering. In addition, there is no recognition that others' points of view are any different from one's own view.Fact|date=October 2007 This stage may be viewed as a kind of authoritarianism.Fact|date=October 2007

"Stage two" (self-interest driven) espouses the "what's in it for me" position, right behavior being defined by what is in one's own best interest. Stage two reasoning shows a limited interest in the needs of others, but only to a point where it might further one's own interests, such as "you scratch my back, and I'll scratch yours." In stage two concern for others is not based on loyalty or intrinsic respect. Lacking a perspective of society in the pre-conventional level, this should not be confused with social contract (stage five), as all actions are performed to serve one's own needs or interests. For the stage two theorist, the perspective of the world is often seen as morally relative.

Conventional

The conventional level of moral reasoning is typical of adolescents and adults. Persons who reason in a conventional way judge the morality of actions by comparing these actions to societal views and expectations. The conventional level consists of the third and fourth stages of moral development.

In "Stage three" (interpersonal accord and conformity driven), the self enters society by filling social roles. Individuals are receptive of approval or disapproval from other people as it reflects society's accordance with the perceived role. They try to be a "good boy" or "good girl" to live up to these expectations, having learned that there is inherent value in doing so. Stage three reasoning may judge the morality of an action by evaluating its consequences in terms of a person's relationships, which now begin to include things like respect, gratitude and the 'golden rule'. Desire to maintain rules and authority exists only to further support these social roles. The intentions of actions play a more significant role in reasoning at this stage; 'they mean well...'

In "Stage four" (authority and social order obedience driven), it is important to obey laws, dictums and social conventions because of their importance in maintaining a functioning society. Moral reasoning in stage four is thus beyond the need for individual approval exhibited in stage three; society must learn to transcend individual needs. A central ideal or ideals often prescribe what is right and wrong, such as in the case of fundamentalism. If one person violates a law, perhaps everyone would - thus there is an obligation and a duty to uphold laws and rules. When someone does violate a law, it is morally wrong; culpability is thus a significant factor in this stage as it separates the bad domains from the good ones. Most active members of society remain at Stage four, where morality is still predominantly dictated by an outside force.

Post-Conventional

The post-conventional level, also known as the principled level, consists of stages five and six of moral development. Realization that individuals are separate entities from society now becomes salient. One's own perspective should be viewed before the society. It is due to this 'nature of self before others' that the post-conventional level, especially stage six, is sometimes mistaken for pre-conventional behaviors.

In "Stage five" (social contract driven), individuals are viewed as holding different opinions and values. Along a similar vein, laws are regarded as social contracts rather than rigid dictums. Those that do not promote the general welfare should be changed when necessary to meet "the greatest good for the greatest number of people". This is attained through majority decision, and inevitably compromise. In this way democratic government is ostensibly based on stage five reasoning.

In "Stage six" (universal ethical principles driven), moral reasoning is based on abstract reasoning using universal ethical principles. Laws are valid only insofar as they are grounded in justice, and that a commitment to justice carries with it an obligation to disobey unjust laws. Rights are unnecessary as social contracts are not essential for deontic moral action. Decisions are not met hypothetically in a conditional way but rather categorically in an absolute way (see Immanuel Kant's 'categorical imperative').cite book | author=Kant, Immanuel | title=Groundwork of the Metaphysic of Morals| publisher=Harper and Row Publishers, Inc | year=1964| id=ISBN 0-06-131159-6] This can be done by imagining what one would do being in anyone's shoes, who imagined what anyone would do thinking the same (see John Rawls's 'veil of ignorance').* cite book | last=Rawls | first=John | authorlink=John Rawls | title=A Theory of Justice | publisher=Belkap Press of Harvard University Press | location=Cambridge, MA | year=1971 |id=ISBN 0-674-01772-2 ] The resulting consensus is the action taken. In this way action is never a means but always an end in itself; one acts "because" it is right, and not because it is instrumental, expected, legal or previously agreed upon. While Kohlberg insisted that stage six exists, he had difficulty finding participants who consistently used it. It appears that people rarely reach stage six of Kohlberg's model.

Further stages

In his empirical studies of persons across their life-span, Kohlberg came to notice that some people evidently had undergone moral stage regression. He was faced with the option of either conceding that moral regression could occur, or revising his theory. Kohlberg chose the latter, postulating the existence of sub-stages wherein the emerging stage has not yet been adequately integrated into the personality. In particular Kohlberg noted of a stage 4½ or 4+, which is a transition from stage four to stage five, sharing characteristics of both. In this stage the individual has become disaffected with the arbitrary nature of "law and order" reasoning. Culpability is frequently turned from being defined by society to having society itself be culpable. This stage is often mistaken for the moral relativism of stage two as the individual views the interests of society which conflict with their own choices as relatively and morally wrong. Kohlberg noted that this was often seen in students entering college.

Kohlberg further speculated that a seventh stage may exist (Transcendental Morality or Morality of Cosmic Orientation) which would link religion with moral reasoningcite book | last=Power | first=Clark | coauthors=Lawrence Kohlberg, ed. | title=Essays on Moral Development Vol. I: Philosophy of Moral Development | publisher=Harper & Row |location=San Francisco, CA | year=1981 |id=ISBN 0-06-064760-4 |chapter= Moral Development, Religious Thinking, and the Question of a Seventh Stage ] (see James W. Fowler's stages of faith development).cite book | last=Fowler | first=John | coauthors=T. Hennessey, ed. | title=Values and Moral Development | publisher=Paulist Press |location=New York | year=1976 |chapter= Stages in Faith: The Structural Developmental Approach ] cite book | last=Fowler | first=John | coauthors=S. Keen, ed. | title=Life Maps: Conversations on the Journey of Faith | publisher=Word Books |location=Waco, TX | year=1978 |id=ISBN 0-8499-2848-6 |chapter= Mapping Faith's Structures: A Developmental View ] However, because of Kohlberg's trouble providing empirical evidence for even a sixth stage, he emphasized that most of his conjecture towards a seventh stage was theoretical.

Theoretical assumptions (philosophy)

Kohlberg's theory is not value-neutral. It begins with a stake in certain perspectives in meta-ethics. This includes for instance a view of human nature, and a certain understanding of the form and content of moral reasoning. It holds conceptions of the right and the scope of moral reasoning across societies. Furthermore it includes the relationship between morality and the world, between morality and logical expression, and the role of reason in morality. Finally, it takes a view of the social and mental processes involved in moral reasoning.

The picture of human nature which Kohlberg begins with is the view that humans are inherently communicative and capable of reason as well as possessing a desire to understand others and the world around them. The stages of Kohlberg's model refer to the qualitative moral "reasonings" that people adopt, and thus do not translate directly into praise or blame of the actions or characters of persons. In order to argue that his theory measures moral reasoning and not particular moral conclusions, Kohlberg insists that the "form and structure" of moral arguments is independent of the "content" of the arguments, a position he calls "formalism".

Kohlberg's theory revolves around the notion that justice is the essential feature of moral reasoning. By the same token, justice relies heavily upon the notion of sound reasoning upon principles. Despite being a justice-centered theory of morality, Kohlberg considered it to be compatible with plausible formulations of deontology and eudaimonia.

Kohlberg's theory "understands" values as a critical component of the right. Whatever the right is, for Kohlberg, it must be universally valid across societies (a position known as "moral universalism"): there can be no relativism. Moreover, morals are not natural features of the world; they are prescriptive. Nevertheless, moral judgments can be evaluated in logical terms of truth and falsity.

According to Kohlberg: a person who progresses to a higher stage of moral reasoning cannot skip stages. For example, one cannot jump from being concerned mostly with peer judgments (stage three) to being a proponent of social contracts (stage five). However, when one encounters a moral dilemma and finds one's current level of moral reasoning unsatisfactory, one will look to the next level. Discovery of the limitations of the current stage of thinking drives moral development as each progressive stage is more adequate than the last. This process is "constructive"; it arises through the conscious construction of the actor, and is neither in any meaningful sense a component of the actor's innate dispositions, nor a result of past inductions.

Formal elements

Progress along the stages of development occurs because of the actor's increased competence in both psychologically and socially balancing conflicting value-claims. The name of "justice operation" is given to the process which resolves the dispute between conflicting claims and strikes an equilibrium between them. "Kohlberg" identifies two of these operations in "equality" and "reciprocity", which respectively involve an impartial regard for persons (i.e., irrespective of who the individual persons are), and a regard for the role of personal merit. For Kohlberg, the most adequate result of both operations is "reversibility", where a moral or dutiful act within a particular situation is evaluated in terms of whether or not the act would be satisfactory even if particular persons were to switch roles within the situation (also known colloquially as "moral musical chairs").

Knowledge and learning contribute to moral development. Specifically important are the actor's "view of persons" and their "social perspective level", each of which becomes more complex and mature with each advancing stage. The view of persons can be understood as the actor's grasp of the psychology of other persons; it may be pictured as a spectrum, with stage one having no view of other persons at all, and stage six being entirely sociocentric. Similarly, the social perspective level involves the understanding of the social universe, differing from the view of persons in that it involves a grasp of norms.

Examples of applied moral dilemmas

Kohlberg established the "Moral Judgement Interview" in his original 1958 dissertation. During the roughly 45 minute tape recorded semi-structured interview, the interviewer uses moral dilemmas to determine which stage of moral reasoning a person uses. The dilemmas are fictional short stories that describe situations in which a person has to make a moral decision. The participant is asked a systemic series of open-ended questions, like what they think the right course of action is, as well as justifications as to why certain actions are right or wrong. The form and structure of these replies are scored and not the content; over a set of multiple moral dilemmas an overall score is derived.

Heinz dilemma

A dilemma that Kohlberg used in his original research was the druggist's dilemma: "Heinz Steals the Drug In Europe".

Quotation|A woman was near death from a special kind of cancer. There was one drug that the doctors thought might save her. It was a form of radium that a druggist in the same town had recently discovered. The drug was expensive to make, but the druggist was charging ten times what the drug cost him to produce. He paid $200 for the radium and charged $2,000 for a small dose of the drug. The sick woman's husband, Heinz, went to everyone he knew to borrow the money, but he could only get together about $ 1,000, which is half of what it cost. He told the druggist that his wife was dying and asked him to sell it cheaper or let him pay later. But the druggist said, "No, I discovered the drug and I'm going to make money from it." So Heinz got desperate and broke into the man's store to steal the drug for his wife.

Should Heinz have broken into the laboratory to steal the drug for his wife? Why or why not?

From a theoretical point of view, it is not important what the participant thinks that Heinz should "do". Kohlberg's theory holds that the justification the participant offers is what is significant, the "form" of their response. Below are some of many examples of possible arguments that belong to the six stages:

Stage one ("obedience"): Heinz should not steal the medicine because he will consequently be put in prison which will mean he is a bad person. Or: Heinz should steal the medicine because it is only worth $200 and not how much the druggist wanted for it; Heinz had even offered to pay for it and was not stealing anything else.

Stage two ("self-interest"): Heinz should steal the medicine because he will be much happier if he saves his wife, even if he will have to serve a prison sentence. Or: Heinz should not steal the medicine because prison is an awful place, and he would probably languish over a jail cell more than his wife's death.

Stage three ("conformity"): Heinz should steal the medicine because his wife expects it; he wants to be a good husband. Or: Heinz should not steal the drug because stealing is bad and he is not a criminal; he tried to do everything he could without breaking the law, you cannot blame him.

Stage four ("law-and-order"): Heinz should not steal the medicine because the law prohibits stealing, making it illegal. Or: Heinz should steal the drug for his wife but also take the prescribed punishment for the crime as well as paying the druggist what he is owed. Criminals cannot just run around without regard for the law; actions have consequences.

Stage five ("human rights"): Heinz should steal the medicine because everyone has a right to choose life, regardless of the law. Or: Heinz should not steal the medicine because the scientist has a right to fair compensation. Even if his wife is sick, it does not make his actions right.

Stage six ("universal human ethics"): Heinz should steal the medicine, because saving a human life is a more fundamental value than the property rights of another person. Or: Heinz should not steal the medicine, because others may need the medicine just as badly, and their lives are equally significant.

Criticisms

One criticism of Kohlberg's theory is that it emphasizes justice to the exclusion of other values. As a consequence of this, it may not adequately address the arguments of people who value other moral aspects of actions. Carol Gilligan has argued that Kohlberg's theory is overly androcentric.cite journal | last=Gilligan | first=Carol | title= | journal=Harvard Educational Review | volume=47 | issue=4 | year=1977 ] Kohlberg's theory was initially developed based on empirical research using only male participants; Gilligan argued that it did not adequately describe the concerns of women. Although research has generally found no significant pattern of differences in moral development between sexes, Gilligan's theory of moral development does not focus on the value of justice. She developed an alternative theory of moral reasoning that is based on the ethics of caring. Critics such as Christina Hoff Sommers, however, argued that Gilligan's research is ill-founded, and that no evidence exists to support her conclusion. [Sommers, "The War Against Boys".]

Other psychologists have questioned the assumption that moral action is primarily reached by formal reasoning. One such group, the social intuitionists, state people often make moral judgments without weighing concerns such as fairness, law, human rights and abstract ethical values. Given this, the arguments that Kohlberg and other rationalist psychologists have analyzed could be considered "post hoc" rationalizations of intuitive decisions. This would mean that moral reasoning is less relevant to moral action than Kohlberg's theory suggests.

Continued relevance

Theory and research of Kohlberg's stages of moral development have been utilized by others in academia. One such example, the "Defining Issues Test" or "DIT", was created by James Rest in 1979cite journal |last=Rest |first=James |authorlink=James Rest |coauthors=Narvaez, D., Bebeau, M. and Thoma, S. |year=1999 |title=DIT-2: Devising and testing a new instrument of moral judgment |journal=Journal of Educational Psychology |volume=91 |issue=4 |pages=644–659 |doi=10.1037/0022-0663.91.4.644 ] Heavily influenced by the six-stage model, it made efforts to improve validity criteria by using a quantitative test of a Likert scale to rate moral dilemmas similar to Kohlberg's.cite web |url=http://www.centerforthestudyofethicaldevelopment.net/DIT%20--Sample%20Dilemma.htm |title=Center for the Study of Ethical Development |accessdate=2006-12-05 |format=Website |work=DIT --Sample Dilemma: Heinz and the Drug ] It also used a large body of Kohlbergian theory such as the idea of 'post-conventional thinking'.cite journal |last=Rest |first=James |authorlink=James Rest |coauthors=Narvaez, D., Bebeau, M. and Thoma, S. |year=1999 |title=A Neo-Kohlbergian Approach: The DIT and Schema Theory |journal=Educational Psychology Review |volume=11 |issue=4 |pages=291–324 |doi=10.1023/A:1022053215271 ] cite book |last=Rest |first=James |authorlink=James Rest |coauthors=Narvaez, D., Bebeau, M. and Thoma, S. |title=Postconventional Moral Thinking: A Neo-Kohlbergian Approach |year=1999 |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |location=Mahwah, NJ |id=ISBN 0-8058-3285-8 ] In 1999 the "DIT" was revised as the "DIT-2"; the test persists in many areas that require moral testingcite book |last=Rest |first=James |authorlink=James Rest |coauthors=Barnett, R., Bebeau, M., Deemer, D., Getz, I., Moon, Y., Spickelmeier, J. Thoma, S. and Volker, J |title=Moral development: Advances in research and theory |year=1986 |publisher=Praeger Publishers |id=ISBN 0-275-92254-5] and in varied cohorts.cite journal |last=Bunch |first=Wilton H. |year=2005|title=Changing moral judgement in divinity students |journal=Journal of Moral Education |volume=34 |issue=3 |pages=363–370|doi=10.1080/03057240500211543 ] cite journal |last=Muhlberger |first=P. |year=2000 |title=Moral reasoning effects on political participation |journal=Political Psychology |volume=21 |issue=4 |pages=667–695 |doi=10.1111/0162-895X.00212 ] cite journal |last=Hedl |first=John J.|coauthors=Glazer, H. and Chan, F. |year=2005 |title=Improving the Moral Reasoning of Allied Health Students |journal=Journal of Allied Health |volume=34 |issue=2 |pages=121–122 ]

ee also

*Jean Piaget, Theory of cognitive development
*Carol Gilligan, Ethics of care
*James W. Fowler, Stages of faith development
*Jane Loevinger, Stages of ego development
*Erik Erikson, Stages of psychosocial development
*James Rest, Defining Issues Test

References

Further reading

*
*
*
*
*

External links

* [http://tigger.uic.edu/~lnucci/MoralEd/overview.html Moral Development and Moral Education: An Overview]
* [http://www.haverford.edu/psych/ddavis/p109g/kohlberg.stages.html Kohlberg's Moral Stages] :)
* [http://bostonreview.net/BR30.5/saxe.html "Boston Review" article covering the topic and other related areas]
* [http://faculty.plts.edu/gpence/html/kohlberg.htm Kohlberg's Stages of Moral Development]


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Нужно сделать НИР?

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Lawrence Kohlberg's stages of moral development — constitute an adaptation of a psychological theory originally conceived of by the Swiss psychologist Jean Piaget. Kohlberg began work on this topic while a psychology postgraduate student at the University of Chicago,[1] and expanded and… …   Wikipedia

  • Stages of faith development — A series of stages of faith development was proposed by Professor James W. Fowler, a developmental psychologist at Candler School of Theology, in the book Stages of Faith . This book length study contains a framework and ideas, which have… …   Wikipedia

  • Loevinger's stages of ego development — Jane Loevinger s stages of ego development includes nine sequential stages, each of which represents a progressively more complex way of perceiving oneself in relation to the world.Infancy* Presocial * No ego * Not Differentiated from the World * …   Wikipedia

  • Moral psychology — is a field of study in both philosophy and psychology. Some use the term moral psychology relatively narrowly to refer to the study of moral development.[1] However, others tend to use the term more broadly to include any topics at the… …   Wikipedia

  • Moral reasoning — is a study in psychology that overlaps with moral philosophy. It is also called moral development. Prominent contributors to theory include Lawrence Kohlberg and Elliot Turiel. The term is sometimes used in a different sense: reasoning under… …   Wikipedia

  • Kohlberg — is the name of four municipalities *Kohlberg, Baden Württemberg in the district Esslingen, Germany *Kohlberg, Bavaria in the district Neustadt (Waldnaab), Germany *Kohlberg (Pirna), in Saxony *Kohlberg, Styria in Styria, AustriaKohlberg was the… …   Wikipedia

  • Stages of development — A stage of development may refer to: Biology: Prenatal development, also called fetal development, or embryology. Human development (biology) Psychology: Developmental stage theories Child development stages stages of child development Erikson s… …   Wikipedia

  • Moral obligation — The term moral obligation has a number of meanings in moral philosophy, in religion, and in layman s terms. Generally speaking, when someone says of an act that it is a moral obligation, they refer to a belief that the act is one prescribed by… …   Wikipedia

  • Lawrence Kohlberg — (October 25, 1927 – January 19, 1987) was an American psychologist born in Bronxville, New York, who served as a professor at the University of Chicago as well as Harvard University. Famous for research in moral education, reasoning, and… …   Wikipedia

  • Piaget's theory of cognitive development — For more information, see Neo Piagetian theories of cognitive development. Piaget s theory of cognitive development is a comprehensive theory about the nature and development of human intelligence first developed by Jean Piaget. It is primarily… …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”