N200 (neuroscience)

N200 (neuroscience)

The N200, or N2, is an event-related potential (ERP) component. An ERP can be monitored using a non-invasive electroencephalography (EEG) cap that is fitted over the scalp on human subjects. An EEG cap allows researchers and clinicians to monitor the minute electrical activity that reaches the surface of the scalp from post-synaptic potentials in neurons, which fluctuate in relation to cognitive processing. EEG provides millisecond-level temporal resolution and is therefore known as one of the most direct measures of covert mental operations in the brain. The N200 in particular is a negative-going wave that peaks 200-350ms post-stimulus and is found primarily over anterior scalp sites [1]. Past research focused on the N200 as a mismatch detector, but it has also been found to reflect executive cognitive control functions, and has recently been used in the study of language (Folstein & Van Petten, 2008; Schmitt, Münte, & Kutas, 2000).

Contents

History

The N2 component starts with the discovery of EEG which dates back as early as 1929 with Hans Berger demonstrating the ability to record electrical activity of the brain by simply placing electrodes over the scalp and then amplifying the signal. Later, in 1936, researcher Pauline and Hallowell Davis manipulated events in the environment and recorded the first known ERP's. One of the first experiments to find evidence of an N200 was by Sutton, Braren, and Zubin (1965) [2] when examining the effects of stimulus uncertainty on sensory potentials. In their study, participants were presented with two types of paired stimuli. In the certain condition, a cue stimulus was presented that was predictive of the modality of the target stimulus, which was either clicks or light flashes. In the uncertain condition, the cue stimulus was not predictive and could be followed by either a click or a light flash. The researchers occasionally found a negativity that peaked on average 190ms post-stimulus in the uncertain condition (N200), in addition to a positivity 300ms post-stimulus (P300).

Following the experiment by Sutton et al. (1965), subsequent research further manipulated stimulus uncertainty in an attempt to elicit a more robust N200. The N200 has been found in a variety of different experimental conditions, and is now thought to consist of several subcomponents. The N200 in response to attended or unattended deviant auditory stimuli, similar to what was originally seen in Sutton et al. (1965), is referred to as the mismatch negativity, or Mismatch negativity. Additionally, there is the no-go N200, which is elicited on no-go trials in go/no-go tasks [3]. More generally, the N2 component has been described in tasks that reflect stimulus identification [4], attentional shifts [4], inhibition of motor responses, overcoming stereotypical responses or conflict monitoring [5], maintenance of context information [5], response selection timing [6], and detection of novelty or mismatch [7].

Main Paradigms

The N200 is seen in a variety of experimental paradigms. A commonly used experimental design is the Eriksen flanker task. In this task, participants are shown an array of items (usually letters), with each letter corresponding to a left or right-handed response. For example, the letter 'A' could indicate a left-handed response, and the letter 'B' a right-handed response. It is the job of the participants to respond to the central item of the array, which is flanked by the same item on compatible trials (AAAAA) or a different item on incompatible trials (BBABB). The N200 is normally seen on incompatible trials.

Another task that has been utilized to elicit a N200 is the go/no-go task [8]. This task presents participants with two different stimuli that indicate which hand to respond with (e.g. 'A' indicates a left-handed response and 'B' a right-handed response). The stimuli also vary on another dimension that indicates whether a response is necessary (e.g. small letter requires a response, large letter means do not respond). For example, a small 'A' would indicate a left-handed go, and a large 'B' would be a right-handed no-go. The go/no-go mapping is then reversed to test for differences (e.g. letter size would indicate the hand and letter identity the go/no-go). The N200 is most often seen on no-go trials.

N200 in the Study of Language

Since the N200 can be used to determine the order of information extraction in the go/no-go task, it is a good candidate to examine language processing and production. Schmitt et al. (2000)[9] utilized the no-go N200 to determine the temporal processing of semantic and phonological information. Participants completed a go/no-go task with semantics (determining whether a picture was an animal or object) mapped to response type and phonology (whether the pictured item began with a vowel or consonant) as the go/no-go. They found that the peak latency of the N200 occurred earlier when the response was contingent on semantic information than when it depended on phonological information. Thus, the researchers were able to conclude that semantic information becomes available before phonological information in language production.

Functional Sensitivity

The latency, amplitude, and distribution of the N200 are sensitive to several factors depending on the type of experiment. The N200 is often seen as part of a complex of components including the P3a and P3b. The N200 component responds functionally much like the P3b component in that stimulus probability can affect the amplitude of both. This is one reason why the P3 and the N2 are often researched together, since they are both sensitive to similar manipulations and represent a connection of mental mechanisms that work together to interpret the changing environment.

In the Eriksen flanker task and go/no-go paradigm, the peak amplitude of the N200 increases for incompatible and for no-go trials respectively [10][11].This increase in amplitude has been hypothesized as the mental need to control incorrect response preparation. Latency is correlated with response time in the flanker task [7]. Although the N200 is primarily distributed over anterior brain regions, posterior distributions have been reported in visual attention paradigms, such as visual search [7].

During a stop signal task the frontocentral N2 is sensitive to time pressure, in that when individuals are asked to respond as quickly as possible the amplitude of the N2 increases. This increase in amplitude is larger within individuals who have what is considered a fast stop signal reaction time and thus who are able to inhibit a preponent response very quickly. The N2 amplitude is also reduced over right frontal electrodes sites in ADHD children [5]. The N2 latency during the stop signal task is longer in unsuccessful than successful trials suggesting that the mental process is taking too long to evaluate the stop signal and therefore not fully processing the signal enough to inhibit a motor response [5].

Component Characteristics

The N2 ERP component can be further divided into three different sub-components: N2a or auditory MMN, N2b, and N2c. Please refer to the outline table below for each sub-component and the outlined differences and similarities.

Component N2a or Auditory MMN N2b N2c
General Location: Frontocentral/Anterior Frontocentral/Anterior Posterior
Co-component: ---- Observed along with frontal P3a component Observed along with P3b component
Attention: Attention not required in repetitive stimulus presentation Requires conscious stimulus attention Requires conscious stimulus attention
Scalp Distribution: Anterior scalp distribution for auditory stimuli. Central scalp distribution for auditory and visual stimuli. Posterior scalp distribution for visual stimuli. Frontal-central distribution for auditory stimuli
Neural Generator: Auditory cortical region, frontal lobe, and possibly hippocampus. Anterior cingulated cortex [7], frontal and superior temporal cortex [4]. ----
Paradigms: Auditory Oddball and Go-Nogo with no behavior response. Oddball, Flanker, Go-Nogo, Stop Signal. Oddball, Flanker, Go-Nogo, Stop Signal.
Latency: Increase latency with increase in non-targets. Related to auditory processing duration [4]. Latency related to timing of mental access to properties of a stimulus. Latency related to reaction time.
Amplitude: Related to sensory auditory memory traces. Larger for non-targets with no behavioral response. Sometimes called the 'no-go N2'. Larger for targets. Much more prominent than N2b.
Probability: ---- Increase in amplitude when no-go trials with the non-targets are less probable or equal with largest effect at Fz. Decreased in amplitude in trials with the high probable targets with largest effect at sites Fz and Cz.
Cognitive Representation: Responsible for detection of novelty or mismatch to the attended stimuli. Believed to reflect disparity between the deviating stimulus and a sensory-memory representation of the standard stimulus. Analyze the characterization of auditory stimulus features in sensory memory [4]. Automatic novelty sensing process [4]. Related to response inhibition, response conflict, and error monitoring. Sensitive to detection of perceptual novelty or attentional deviation. Overriding a preponent response system. Deviation from a mentally-stored expectation of the standard stimulus [4]. Represents visual attention or degree of attention that is needed for processing of stimuli context and features within the visual cortex of the brain [7].

Theory/Sources

In go/no-go tasks, no-go trials require inhibition of a response when information indicating response hand is processed before the go/no-go information. Presence of an N200 on no-go trials suggests that the N200 reflects a cognitive control function, specifically an inhibitory response control mechanism [7].

However, the theory of the N200 as a response-inhibition mechanism has been debated by Donkers and van Boxtel (2004) [12]. They compared ERP recordings from a go/no-go task to a go/GO task, where the GO was a more forceful response to the go task. This experimental set-up allowed them to compare the no-go task, where some responses are inhibited and compete with one another, with the GO task, where responses just compete. Evidence of a N200 was present in both the no-go and GO task, so the researchers reasoned that the N200 does not represent response-inhibition, but rather conflict monitoring. However, it is still clear that the N200 represents some cognitive control function.


See also

References

  1. ^ Folstein, J. R., & Van Petten, C. (2008). Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: A review. Psychophysiology, 45, 152-170.
  2. ^ Sutton, S., Braren, M., & Zubin, J. (1965). Evoked-potential correlates of stimulus uncertainty. Science, 150, 1187-1188.
  3. ^ Pfefferbaum, A., Ford, J. M., Weller, B. J., & Kopell, B. S. (1985). ERPs to response production and inhibition. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 60, 423-434.
  4. ^ a b c d e f g Patel, S. H., & Azzam, P. N. (2005). Characterization of N200 and P300: Selected studies of the event related potential. International Journal of Medical Sciences , 2, 147-154.
  5. ^ a b c d Azizian, A., Freitas, A. L., Parvaz, M. A., & Squires, N. K. (2006). Beware misleading cues: Perceptual similarity modulates the N2/P3 complex. Psychophysiology , 43, 253-260.
  6. ^ Gajewski, P. D., Stoerig, P., & Falkenstein, M. (2008). ERP - Correlates of response selection in a response conflict paradigm. Brain Research , 1189, 127-134.
  7. ^ a b c d e f Folstein, J. R., & Van Petten, C. (2008). Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: A review. Psychophysiology , 45, 152-170.
  8. ^ Heil, M., Osman, A., Wiegelmann, J., Rolke, B., & Henninghausen, E., (2000). N200 in the Eriksen-task: Inhibitory executive process?. Journal of Psychophysiology, 14, 218-225.
  9. ^ Schmitt, B. M., Münte, T. F., & Kutas, M. (2000). Electrophysiological estimates of the time course of semantic and phonological encoding during implicit picture naming. Psychophysiology, 37, 473-484
  10. ^ Heil, M., Osman, A., Wiegelmann, J., Rolke, B., & Henninghausen, E., (2000). N200 in the Eriksen-task: Inhibitory executive process?. Journal of Psychophysiology, 14, 218-225.
  11. ^ Pfefferbaum, A., Ford, J. M., Weller, B. J., & Kopell, B. S. (1985). ERPs to response production and inhibition. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 60, 423-434
  12. ^ Donkers, F. C. L., & van Boxtel, G. J. M. (2004). The N2 in go/no-go tasks reflects conflict monitoring not response inhibition. Brain and Cognition, 56, 165-176

Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Нужно сделать НИР?

Look at other dictionaries:

  • N400 (neuroscience) — The N400 is a component of time locked EEG signals known as event related potentials (ERP). It is a negative going deflection that peaks around 400 milliseconds post stimulus onset, although it can extend from 250 500 ms, and is typically maximal …   Wikipedia

  • P300 (neuroscience) — The P300 (P3) wave is an event related potential (ERP) elicited by infrequent, task relevant stimuli. It is considered to be an endogenous potential as its occurrence links not to the physical attributes of a stimulus but to a person s reaction… …   Wikipedia

  • N2 — Contents 1 Transportation Military 2 Roads 3 Other 4 See also N2, N 2 or N02 may be …   Wikipedia

  • P200 — In neuroscience, the visual P200 or P2 is a waveform component or feature of the event related potential (ERP) measured at the human scalp. Like other potential changes measurable from the scalp, this effect is believed to reflect the post… …   Wikipedia

  • P600 — This article is about the event related potential. For the mobile phone, see Sendo P600. For the molecule, see Interleukin 13. The P600 is an event related potential (ERP), or peak in electrical brain activity measured by electroencephalography… …   Wikipedia

  • Mismatch negativity — Mismatch field and MMNM redirect here. The mismatch negativity (MMN) or mismatch field (MMF) is a component of the event related potential (ERP) to an odd stimulus in a sequence of stimuli. It arises from electrical activity in the brain and is… …   Wikipedia

  • Evoked potential — Intervention MeSH D005071 An evoked potential (or evoked response ) is an electrical potential recorded from the nervous system of a human or other animal following presentation of a …   Wikipedia

  • Difference due to Memory — (Dm) indexes differences in neural activity during the study phase of an experiment for items that subsequently are remembered compared to items that are later forgotten. It is mainly discussed as an event related potential (ERP) effect that… …   Wikipedia

  • N170 — The N170 is a component of the event related potential that reflects the neural processing of faces. When potentials evoked by images of faces are compared to those elicited by other visual stimuli, the former show increased negativity 130 200 ms …   Wikipedia

  • Bereitschaftspotential — In neurology, the Bereitschaftspotential or BP (from German, readiness potential ), also called the pre motor potential or readiness potential (RP), is a measure of activity in the motor cortex of the brain leading up to voluntary muscle movement …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”