Illusion of control

Illusion of control

Illusion of control is the tendency for human beings to believe they can control, or at least influence, outcomes that they demonstrably have no influence over.

Experimental demonstration

One kind of demonstration involves a set-up with two lights marked "Score" and "No Score". Subjects have to try to control which one switches on. Jenkins & Ward (1965) presented subjects with two buttons to press. Allan & Jenkins (1980) had one button, which subjects decided on each trial to press or not press. The connections could be arranged so that each action switches on one light with a given probability, so subjects had zero control over which light is on, or a variable amount of control. Subjects were told that there might be no relation between their actions and the lights.

Subjects were asked to estimate how much control they had over the lights. Their estimates bore no relation to how much control they actually had, but was related to how often the "Score" light lit up. Even when it made no difference what they chose, subjects confidently reported exerting some control over the lights.

In a series of experiments, Ellen Langer (1975) demonstrated first the prevalence of the illusion of control and second, that people were more likely to behave as if they could exercise control in a chance situation where “skill cues” were present. By skill cues, Langer meant properties of the situation more normally associated with the exercise of skill, in particular the exercise of choice, competition, familiarity with the stimulus and involvement in decisions.

One simple form of this fallacy is found in casinos: when rolling dice in craps, it has been shown that people tend to throw harder for high numbers and softer for low numbers. Under some circumstances, experimental subjects have been induced to believe that they could affect the outcome of a purely random coin toss. Subjects who guessed a series of coin tosses more successfully began to believe that they were actually better guessers, and believed that their guessing performance would be less accurate if they were distracted.

An illusion of control over certain external events could be a basis for belief in psychokinesis.

Interpretation of the phenomenon

Taylor & Brown (1988), have argued that positive illusions are adaptive as they increase motivation and persistence. This position is supported by Albert Bandura's claim that ‘optimistic self-appraisals of capability, that are not unduly disparate from what is possible, can be advantageous, whereas veridical judgements can be self-limiting’ (Bandura, 1989, p.1177). We should, however, note here Bandura’s use of the qualification ‘not unduly disparate from what is possible’. His argument is essentially concerned with the adaptive effect of optimistic beliefs about control and performance in circumstances where control is possible, rather than perceived control in circumstances where outcomes are genuinely non-contingent on an individual’s behaviour.

Bandura has also suggested that: "In activities where the margins of error are narrow and missteps can produce costly or injurious consequences, personal well-being is best served by highly accurate efficacy appraisal." (1997, p. 71)

Taylor and Brown argue that positive illusions are adaptive, since there is evidence that they are more common in normally mentally healthy individuals than in depressed individuals. However, Pacini, Muir and Epstein (1998) have shown that this may be because depressed people overcompensate for a tendency toward maladaptive intuitive processing by exercising excessive rational control in trivial situations, and note that the difference with non-depressed people disappears in more consequential circumstances.

There is also empirical evidence that high self-efficacy can be maladaptive in some circumstances. In a scenario-based study, Whyte et al. (1997) showed that participants in whom they had induced high self-efficacy were significantly more likely to escalate commitment to a failing course of action. Knee and Zuckerman (1998) have challenged the definition of mental health used by Taylor and Brown and argue that lack of illusions is associated with a non-defensive personality oriented towards growth and learning and with low ego involvement in outcomes. They present evidence that self-determined individuals are less prone to these illusions. In the late 1970s, Abramson and Alloy (1980) demonstrated that depressed individuals hold a more accurate view of their control of the social environment than do non-depressed individuals. This finding holds true even when the depression is manipulated experimentally.

Fenton-O'Creevy et al (2003) argue, as do Gollwittzer and Kinney (1989), that while illusory beliefs about control may promote goal striving, they are not conducive to sound decision-making. Illusions of control may cause insensitivity to feedback, impede learning and predispose toward greater objective risk taking (since subjective risk will be reduced by illusion of control).

In a study of the illusion of control in a population of traders working in investment banks, Fenton-O'Creevy et al (2003, 2004) found that traders who were prone to high illusion of control had significantly worse performance on analysis, risk management and contribution to desk profits. They also earned significantly less.

One important explanation for illusion of control may lie in self-regulation theory. To the extent that people are driven by internal goals concerned with the exercise of control over their environment, they will seek to reassert control in conditions of chaos, uncertainty or stress. Failing genuine control, one coping strategy will be to fall back on defensive attributions of control—leading to illusions of control (Fenton-O'Creevy et al, 2003).

ee also

* Clustering illusion
* Gambler's fallacy
* Inverse gambler's fallacy
* List of cognitive biases
* Magical thinking
* Princeton Engineering Anomalies Research Lab

Further reading

* Baron, J. (2000). "Thinking and deciding" (3d. edition). New York: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-65030-5

References

* Allan, L. G. & Jenkins, H. M. (1980). The judgment of contingency and the nature of the response alternatives. "Canadian Journal of Psychology", 34, 1-11.
* Bandura, A. (1989). Human Agency in Social Cognitive Theory. "American Psychologist", 44(9), 1175-1184.
* Bandura, A. (1997). "Self-efficacy: The exercise of control". New York: W.H. Freeman and Company.
* Fenton-O’Creevy, M., Nicholson, N. and Soane, E., Willman, P. " Traders - Risks, Decisions, and Management in Financial Markets " ISBN 0-19-926948-3
* Fenton-O’Creevy, M., Nicholson, N. and Soane, E., Willman, P. (2003) Trading on illusions: Unrealistic perceptions of control and trading performance. "Journal of Occupational and Organisational Psychology" 76, 53-68.
* Gollwitzer, P. M., & Kinney, R. F. (1989). Effects of Deliberative and Implemental Mind-Sets On Illusion of Control. "Journal of Personality and Social Psychology", 56(4), 531-542.
* Henslin, J. M. (1967). Craps and magic. "American Journal of Sociology" 73, 316-330.
* Jenkins, H. H. & Ward, W. C. (1965) Judgement of contingency between responses and outcomes. "Psychological Monographs", 79 (1, Wole No. 79).
* Knee, C. R., & Zuckerman, M. (1998). A nondefensive personality: Autonomy and control as moderators of defensive coping and self-handicapping. "Journal of Research in Personality", 32(2), 115-130.
* Langer, E. J. (1975). The Illusion of Control. "Journal of Personality and Social Psychology" 32(2), 311-328.
* Langer, E. J. & Roth, J. (1975). Heads I win, tails it's chance: The illusion of control as a function of the sequence of outcomes in a purely chance task. "Journal of Personality and Social Psychology" 32(6), 951-955.
* Langer, E. J. (1982). The Illusion of Control. In Kahneman D., Slovic P., & Tversky, A. (Eds.). "Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases". New York: Cambridge University Press
* Pacini, R., Muir, F., & Epstein, S. (1998). Depressive realism from the perspective of cognitive-experiential self-theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(4), 1056-1068.
* Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and Well-Being - a Social Psychological Perspective On Mental-Health. "Psychological Bulletin", 103(2), 193-210.
* Whyte, G., Saks, A. & Hook, S. (1997) When success breeds failure: The role of self-efficacy in escalating commitment to a losing course of action. "Journal of Organizational Behavior", 18, 415-432.


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем написать курсовую

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Illusion (comics) — Superherobox| caption= comic color=background:#ff8080 character name=Illusion real name=Ilya Zarkov species= publisher=Marvel Comics debut= The Vision and the Scarlet Witch #4 (Jan 1986) creators=Steve Englehart and Richard Howell alliance… …   Wikipedia

  • illusion — illusioned, adj. /i looh zheuhn/, n. 1. something that deceives by producing a false or misleading impression of reality. 2. the state or condition of being deceived; misapprehension. 3. an instance of being deceived. 4. Psychol. a perception, as …   Universalium

  • illusion —    by William Pawlett   Illusion and its cognates appearance, image, double, mirror, simulation, shadow are terms woven throughout Baudrillard s writings, from his earliest studies of consumption to his last provocations on evil and the dual form …   The Baudrillard dictionary

  • Introspection illusion — The introspection illusion is a cognitive illusion in which people wrongly think they have direct insight into the origins of their mental states, while treating others introspections as unreliable. In certain situations, this illusion leads… …   Wikipedia

  • Ilusión del control — es la tendencia de los seres humanos de creer que pueden controlar, o al menos influir, los resultados en los que claramente no tienen ninguna influencia. Contenido 1 Demostración experimental 2 Interpretación del fenómeno 3 Véase también …   Wikipedia Español

  • World of Illusion Starring Mickey Mouse and Donald Duck — friends International box art Developer(s) Sega (AM7) …   Wikipedia

  • Lady Illusion — is a hero/villain from the BBC programme, Ace Lightning. Originally programmed to be Lord Fear s sidekick, Lady Illusion quickly abandoned her programming and fell in love with Ace, who had never experienced emotions before. Despite the fact she… …   Wikipedia

  • Motion-Control-Fotografie — (Fotografie mit Bewegungssteuerung) ist eine in Filmen genutzte Spezialeffekt Technik, die es ermöglicht, dieselben Kamera oder Objektbewegungen durch computergestützte Automatisierung in verschiedenen Aufnahmen genau zu wiederholen. In der… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Magic (illusion) — For other varieties of magic, see Magic (disambiguation). Illusionist redirects here. For the artistic tradition, see Illusionism (art). For other uses, see The Illusionist (disambiguation). Magic The Conjurer (painting), 1475 1480, by Hieronymus …   Wikipedia

  • Motion Control-Fotografie — (Fotografie mit Bewegungssteuerung) ist eine in Filmen genutzte Spezialeffekt Technik, die es ermöglicht, dieselben Kamera oder Objektbewegungen durch computergestützte Automatisierung in verschiedenen Aufnahmen genau zu wiederholen. In der… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”